The effectiveness of celebrity endorsements: a meta-analysis

  • Original Empirical Research
  • Open access
  • Published: 12 October 2016
  • Volume 45 , pages 55–75, ( 2017 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

celebrity endorsement thesis

  • Johannes Knoll 1 &
  • Jörg Matthes   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-9408-955X 1  

191k Accesses

58 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

Celebrities frequently endorse products, brands, political candidates, or health campaigns. We investigated the effectiveness of such endorsements by meta-analyzing 46 studies published until April 2016 involving 10,357 participants. Applying multilevel meta-analysis, we analyzed celebrity endorsements in the context of for-profit and non-profit marketing. Findings revealed strong positive and negative effects when theoretically relevant moderators were included in the analysis. The most positive attitudinal effect appeared for male actors who match well with an implicitly endorsed object (d = .90). The most negative effect was found for female models not matching well with an explicitly endorsed object (d = −.96). Furthermore, celebrity endorsements performed worse compared to endorsements of quality seals, awards, or endorser brands. No publication bias was detected. The study has theoretical and practical implications, and provides an agenda for future research.

Similar content being viewed by others

celebrity endorsement thesis

Ten years of evolving traditional versus non-traditional celebrity endorser study: review and synthesis

celebrity endorsement thesis

The Meaning Transfer Process in Celebrity Endorsements: A Quantitative Exploration

celebrity endorsement thesis

Don’t Hate me Because I am Beautiful: Identifying the Relative Influence of Celebrity Attractiveness and Character Traits on Credibility

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Celebrity endorsements are a well-established marketing strategy used since the late nineteenth century (Erdogan 1999 ). While the strategy was first applied in traditional brand or product marketing (Erdogan 1999 ), it has spread to any form of marketing communication, including political marketing (Chou 2014 , 2015 ), health communication, and the marketing of non-government organizations (NGOs; *Jackson 2008 ; *Wheeler 2009 ; *Young and Miller 2015 ). Current estimates indicate every fourth to fifth advertisement incorporates this strategy, though this varies across countries (USA: 19–25%, Elberse and Verleun 2012 ; Stephens and Rice 1998 ; UK: 21%, Pringle and Binet 2005 ; India: 24%, Crutchfield 2010 ; Japan: 70%, Kilburn 1998 ; Taiwan: 45%, Crutchfield 2010 ). In addition, longitudinal analyses show a steady increase over the past years (Erdogan 1999 ; Pringle and Binet 2005 ).

Hence, many studies have been conducted to test whether consumer attitudes and behavior are changed by celebrity endorsements. So far, results have been summarized in three narrative (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ; Erdogan 1999 ; Kaikati 1987 ) and one quantitative review (Amos et al. 2008 ; see the Appendix for a summary of the review). The quantitative review of Amos and colleagues focused on source effects of celebrity endorsers. In short, it asked which source variables (e.g., expertise, attractiveness) exert which influence on advertising effectiveness. However, it did not test whether the obtained effect sizes were significant, but solely tested whether they were significantly different from each other. Hence, up until now, there is no meta-analytic knowledge about whether celebrity endorsements actually influence consumers’ responses, including the size of their influence. In addition, there is no knowledge about whether effects differ in terms of specific outcomes (e.g., cognitive, affective, behavioral). The reason is that Amos et al. ( 2008 ) applied only a combined measure of advertising effectiveness. Furthermore, frequently claimed propositions like the match-up hypothesis or the proposition of stronger effects in the case of unfamiliar brands have never been tested on a meta-analytic level. This seems particularly pressing considering the fact that practitioners frequently refer to such claims (Erdogan 1999 ). Besides, there are conflicting results of individual studies, for instance, when it comes to the endorser’s sex or endorsement repetition (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ; Erdogan 1999 ). Last but not least, numerous studies have been conducted since the last quantitative review in 2004 (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ).

The present meta-analysis seeks to address these shortcomings by integrating research published through April 2016, by reporting average effect sizes according to various advertising outcomes including respective confidence intervals, and by performing moderator analyses testing the impact of various endorser and endorsed object variables. In terms of methodological advancement, we apply multilevel modeling accounting for the dependence of multiple effect sizes and we estimate publication bias, both important issues in meta-analysis (Borenstein et al. 2009 ). Finally, we provide practitioners with empirically derived implications for how to choose the right celebrity and offer researchers an agenda for future research.

Conceptual framework

Following McCracken ( 1989 ), celebrity endorsements are understood as a marketing technique in which an individual enjoying public recognition “uses this recognition on behalf of a consumer good by appearing with it in an advertisement” (p. 310). The effects of endorsements can well be explained within the advertising effectiveness model provided by Ladvidge and Steiner (Lavidge and Steiner 1961 ). Studies have mostly investigated celebrity endorsements according to one or more of the model’s advertising functions (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ; Erdogan 1999 ; Kaikati 1987 ). Furthermore, the model has revealed itself as fruitful in a similar meta-analysis (Grewal et al. 1997 ). It enables a systematic organization of the analyzed dependent variables and moderators, and specifies their relationships (see Fig. 1 ). According to the model, advertising serves to influence three basic psychological dimensions: the cognitive, the affective, and the conative. “Advertising’s cognitive function provides information and facts for the purpose of making consumers aware and knowledgeable about the sponsored brand. Advertising’s affective function creates liking and preference for the sponsored brand – preference presumably refers to more favorable attitudes. Advertising’s affective function, therefore, is to persuade. Finally, advertising’s conative function is to stimulate desire and cause consumers to buy the sponsored brand” (Grewal et al. 1997 , p. 2). Important to note, we do not suggest that these outcomes necessarily take place in a particular sequence (i.e., cognition = > affect = > behavior). Following more recent advancements in the conceptualization of advertising effects, we propose that each of the outcomes may be independently influenced by celebrity endorsements. In addition, all outcomes are assumed to be interrelated. They possibly influence or interact with each other (Vakratsas and Ambler 1999 ). This is indicated by the double-headed arrows in Fig. 1 .

Celebrity endorsement effectiveness model adapted from Grewal et al. ( 1997 )

Cognitive effects

Cognitive effects include awareness and knowledge about an endorsed object. Establishing awareness starts from creating attention and interest (Lavidge and Steiner 1961 ). Directing one’s attention involves controlled as well as automatic processes (Kahneman 1973 ). Both processes can be influenced by celebrity endorsements. First, people who are interested in a particular celebrity are assumed to purposefully direct their attention to this celebrity’s ad (*Wei and Lu 2013 ). Second, people’s attention is automatically directed. Humans tend to give preferential treatment to stimuli that are related to their goals (Lang 2000 ). In addition, celebrities are well-known, resulting in more accessible representations in memory (Erfgen et al. 2015 ). This should foster automatic attention, too (Bargh and Pratto 1986 ).

Once a celebrity endorsement grabs their attention, consumers are assumed to become more interested in the advertised object as compared with a non-endorsed or other-endorsed object. This due to the fact that celebrities possess inherent news value caused by their celebrity status (Corbett and Mori 1999 ). Since celebrities are generally liked, consumers also tend to be more motivated to assess what kind of object a celebrity is endorsing. As a result, object recall and recognition is assumed to be enhanced due to greater message elaboration (*Petty et al. 1983 ).

In terms of knowledge, celebrity endorsements are assumed to influence the meaning of the endorsed object (*Miller and Allen 2012 ) as well as perceptions about its price, its taste level, the risk of buying it, or the perceived information value of the endorsement (*Biswas et al. 2006 ; *Dean and Biswas 2001 ; *Freiden 1982 ; *Friedman et al. 1976 ; *Young and Miller 2015 ). Based on the mechanism behind these effects, consumers are assumed to conclude that an object has a specific attribute when they perceive this object as paired with a celebrity known for this attribute (e.g., premium price with a high-class celebrity; *Miller and Allen 2012 ). The process can be conceptualized as propositional learning (De Houwer 2009 ). Consumers have experienced in their past that people frequently present themselves with objects they share similarities with (Elliot and Wattanasuwan 1998 ). “Once a relation between two events has been discovered in the past, it is likely that this knowledge is used to generate propositions about similar events in the present” (De Houwer 2009 , p. 8). As a result, celebrity attributes created through celebrities’ role in society transfer to associated objects (McCracken 1989 ). In conclusion, it is proposed that celebrity endorsements influence consumers’ cognitions including attention and interest, awareness, as well as perceptions.

H1: As compared with non-celebrity endorsements or no endorsements, celebrity endorsements evoke greater attention, interest, and awareness as well as perceptions more in line with the respective endorser.

Affective effects

Affective effects pertain to attitudes toward the ad and attitudes toward the advertised object. This influence may best be explained with regard to balance theory (Heider 1946 , 1958 ; see also Mowen and Brown 1981 ). The theory explains a person’s desire to maintain consistency among a triad of linked cognitions. It follows that people generally strive for a consistent organization of their cognitive structures, experiencing this state as most tension-free. In the case of celebrity endorsements, the cognitive triad consists of the consumer, the celebrity, and the endorsed object or the endorsed ad, respectively. A consistent state is achieved if the consumer perceives the celebrity and the endorsed object/ad as equally valenced (i.e., as both positive or both negative) because celebrities endorsing an object are usually seen as positively related to that object or the respective ad (Erdogan 1999 ). Starting from the premise that researchers and practitioners usually employ likeable celebrities, it can be hypothesized that celebrity endorsements positively impact consumers’ attitudes toward the ad and attitudes toward the endorsed object. Only then are consumers’ attitudes and their liking for the respective celebrity of the same valence (i.e., both positive; Heider 1946 , 1958 ). Although there may be similar effects for likeable non-celebrity endorsers, these are assumed to be notably weaker. This is due to the fact that consumers are familiar with celebrities by definition. As a result, relationships with celebrities are more affectional as compared with unknown non-celebrity endorsers (Dibble et al. 2016 ).

H2: As compared with non-celebrity endorsements or no endorsements, celebrity endorsements evoke more positive attitudes toward the ad and the endorsed object.

Behavioral effects

Behavioral effects include purchasing or using an object (e.g., *Freiden 1982 ; *Kamins 1989 ; *Kamins and Gupta 1994 ; *Roozen and Claeys 2010 ; *Siemens et al. 2008 ), sharing object information, volunteering, supporting a charitable cause, or voting for a political candidate (Myrick and Evans 2014 ; *Pease and Brewer 2008 ; *Wei and Lu 2013 ; *Wheeler 2009 ). Such effects are frequently explained with regard to the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1991 ). According to the theory, behavior is strongly determined by behavioral intentions. These are, in turn, influenced by consumers’ attitudes, the perceived subjective norm, and the perceived behavioral control. As long as consumers are able to exert the respective behavior (behavioral control), and as long as consumers do not feel social pressure to avoid the behavior (subjective norm), attitudes largely predict behavioral intentions. The assumptions have been supported by various meta-analyses (Armitage and Conner 2001 ; Kim and Hunter 1993 ). Accordingly, the more positive attitudes assumed in H2 should lead to stronger behavioral intentions and respective behavior. Corresponding effects were, for instance, found by Fleck et al. ( 2012 ) and Mishra and Mishra ( 2014 ). We consequently hypothesize:

H3: As compared with non-celebrity endorsements or no endorsements, celebrity endorsements evoke stronger behavioral intentions and behavior.

Studies investigating the applied advertising effectiveness framework have consistently found that people respond differently to advertisements depending on characteristics of the ad, the advertised object, and individual characteristics (Vakratsas and Ambler 1999 ). This is frequently intended by the advertiser, tailoring advertisements to specific consumers and their needs (Lavidge and Steiner 1961 ). In line with this reasoning, we included various moderators within our framework accounting for the fact that consumers do not respond uniformly to advertising (cf. Figure 1 ; Lavidge and Steiner 1961 ). Following Grewal et al. ( 1997 ), our analysis of moderators is limited to those that (1) are theoretically relevant, (2) provide a sufficient number of effect sizes, (3) show sufficient variance to test the moderation, and (4) are important to advertisers. In terms of number of effect sizes, Higgins and Green ( 2011 ) suggest considering moderator analysis only if there are ten or more studies incorporating the moderators. Seven moderators met the criteria: endorser sex, endorser type, endorser match, endorsement explicitness, endorsement frequency, familiarity of the endorsed object, and endorsement type of the comparison group.

Endorser sex

Though endorser sex has generally been viewed as influential (e.g., Erdogan 1999 ; McCracken 1989 ), hardly any study explicitly addressed this variable in empirical research. Most studies have investigated either female or male endorsers (for the only exception, see Freiden 1984 ). “The dearth of research on endorser gender effects is somewhat surprising as persuasion research shows that men and women respond differently to male and female communicators” (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 , p. 11). Hence, meta-analysis seems especially valuable (Lipsey and Wilson 2001 ). Assumptions about possible effects may be derived from studies on non-celebrity spokespersons. According to Kenton ( 1989 ), the credibility and persuasiveness of a spokesperson depends on four dimensions: goodwill and fairness (e.g., unselfishness), prestige (e.g., power, status), expertise (e.g., competence), and self-presentation (e.g., confidence). Research has revealed women to be higher ranking on goodwill and fairness, whereas men outperform women on the remaining dimensions (Kenton 1989 ). As a result, male spokespersons were frequently more persuasive than female ones (e.g., Cabalero et al. 1989 ; Whittaker 1965 ). Transferring this to the present context, consumers may perceive male celebrity endorsers as more credible due to higher levels of expertise and prestige (Cabalero et al. 1989 ). As a result, male celebrities are assumed to evoke stronger endorsements effects when compared to female ones.

H4: Male celebrity endorsers evoke stronger endorsement effects when compared to female ones.

Endorser type

No study has explicitly investigated different types of celebrity endorsers. Instead, studies have typically focused on only one type. For instance, studies have explored actors, models, musicians, athletes, or TV hosts (e.g., *Dean and Biswas 2001 ; *Frizzell 2011 ; *Pease and Brewer 2008 ; *Wheeler 2009 ; *Wei and Lu 2013 ). By joining the results of several studies, meta-analysis can provide information whether certain endorser types perform better than others do (Lipsey and Wilson 2001 ).

Starting from the premise that endorsement effects depend on the strength of the relationship a consumer shares with a celebrity (McCracken 1989 ), research on parasocial interaction can provide insights. Specifically, studies have revealed that people tend to develop relationships with celebrities, merely known from the media, just as they would do with real life persons (Dibble et al. 2016 ): Upon encountering a celebrity on television, radio, or the Internet, consumers may parasocially interact with the celebrity, storing this experience in a relationship schema (Klimmt et al. 2006 ). The more frequently a celebrity is encountered and the more intense each interaction experience is, the more likely a strong consumer–celebrity relationship is formed (Klimmt et al. 2006 ). Looking at different kinds of celebrities, consumers are particularly likely to form a strong relationship with actors. First, consumers are audiovisually exposed to actors, creating a particularly rich experience, and second, experience is usually based upon multiple encounters over a longer period: “Over time, viewers become familiar with characters and performers on continuing series and often feel as though they know these individuals as well as they know their friends and neighbors. The importance of characters to viewers frequently extends beyond the viewing situation to include the sense of having personal relationships with the characters, deep concern about what happens in their ‘lives,’ and/or a desire to become like them in significant ways” (Hoffner and Buchanan 2005 , p. 326).

According to McCracken ( 1989 ), this exact type of relationship causes consumers to accept celebrities’ influence more readily. The following hypothesis is proposed:

H5: Actors elicit stronger celebrity endorsement effects when compared to other types of celebrities such as models, musicians, athletes, or TV hosts.

Endorser match

Several studies have investigated the so-called product match-up hypothesis that assumes the effectiveness of celebrity endorsements is partially dependent on the degree of perceived fit between an endorsed object and the respective celebrity (Erdogan 1999 ). A good match may be an attractive model presenting cosmetics, whereas a bad match may be an athlete trying to sell a guitar. The process underlying the product match-up hypothesis can be explained with regard to Social Adaptation Theory (Kahle and Homer 1985 ; Kamins 1990 ) or Schema Theory (Lynch and Schuler 1994 ). Social Adaptation Theory assumes that people use information sources as long as they facilitate adaptation to their environment. If a match exists between a spokesperson and a product on some relevant attribute, the spokesperson becomes an information source of adaptive significance on which people may rely (Kamins 1990 ). Schema Theory posits that attributes of celebrities can be integrated more easily with existing product schemas if the celebrity schemas match the product schemas (Lynch and Schuler 1994 ). Both theories assume enhanced effects in the case of congruence. Accordingly, several studies have supported the product match-up hypothesis (Erdogan 1999 ). We thus hypothesize larger effects for object–endorser congruence compared to incongruence.

H6: Congruent celebrity endorsers evoke stronger endorsements effects when compared to incongruent ones.

Endorsement explicitness

Explicitness can broadly be categorized into two modes: implicit and explicit endorsements. Whereas implicit endorsements refer to situations where celebrities simply use an object or merely appear jointly without overtly announcing their support (“I use this object”; *Miller and Allen 2012 ), explicit endorsements refer to situations where celebrities overtly express their support for an object (“I endorse this object”; *Miller and Allen 2012 ). To the best of our knowledge, no study has ever compared both modes; instead, they have researched either implicit or explicit endorsements. Though effects have been found with both modes, one mode may be more effective than the other (implicit: e.g., *Miller and Allen 2012 ; explicit: *Dean and Biswas 2001 ; *Friedman and Friedman 1979 ). According to Russell and Stern ( 2006 ), consumers infer the celebrity–object association to be of greater strength if celebrities explicitly express their support, signaling commitment and reliability. In addition, consumers may not even realize that an object is endorsed if the endorsement is too subtle. We consequently propose that explicit endorsements are more effective that implicit ones.

H7: Explicit endorsements evoke stronger effects than implicit ones.

Endorsement frequency

Celebrities may also vary in their endorsement frequency. Consumers are highly likely to encounter celebrity endorsements multiple times via various media channels, including TV, billboards, print advertising, radio, and the Internet. Research on classical conditioning suggests that effects may occur as early as a single pairing of a celebrity with an endorsed object (e.g., Ambroise et al. 2014 ; Gorn 1982 ). However, other research suggests that effects tend to be greater the greater the number of pairings. For instance, Stuart et al. ( 1987 ) increased the number of pairings from one to three, to ten, and eventually to twenty, revealing a steady increase in effectiveness. Although these results do not directly refer to celebrity endorsements, similar effects can be assumed because celebrity endorsements are often seen as a certain type of classical conditioning (e.g., *Chen et al. 2012 ). The following hypothesis is proposed:

H8: Celebrity endorsement effects increase with increased endorsement exposure.

Familiarity of the endorsed object

Next to the celebrity, the endorsed object itself may impact endorsement effectiveness (*Friedman and Friedman 1979 ). For instance, researchers assume stronger effects, with decreasing familiarity with an endorsed object (*Miller and Allen 2012 ). Object familiarity can be understood as the number of object-related experiences accumulated by a consumer (Alba and Hutchinson 1987 ). These experiences can be obtained directly and indirectly, such as through celebrity endorsements (Kent and Allen 1994 ). The more familiar a person is with an object, the more comprehensive his or her knowledge structures can become (Keller 2012 ). Given that consumers already possess a rich network of associations representing an object, attitudes, and behavior appear more difficult to change (Cacioppo et al. 1992 ). Accordingly, Ambroise et al. ( 2014 ) reported stronger celebrity endorsement effects with unfamiliar compared to familiar brands. Similarly, Shimp et al. ( 1991 ) showed the likelihood of conditioning effects for unknown or moderately known objects, but not for well-known ones. We consequently propose stronger celebrity endorsement effects for unfamiliar objects when compared to familiar ones.

H9: Celebrity endorsement effects are stronger for unfamiliar objects when compared to familiar ones.

Endorsement type of the comparison group

Investigating the effectiveness of celebrity endorsements through experiments, researchers have chosen various control groups. Frequently, celebrities are compared with a non-endorsed condition (e.g., *Martín-Santana and Beerli-Palacio 2013 ), an expert (e.g., Biswas et al. 2006 ), or an ordinary consumer (e.g., *Dong 2015 ). Less frequently, celebrities are compared with an unknown model or athlete (e.g., *Roozen and Claeys 2010 ), an employee of the selling company (*Maronick 2005 ), a quality seal or an award (*Dean and Biswas 2001 ), or an endorser brand from the same product category (*Sengupta et al. 1997 ). Studies typically apply one or two of these comparison groups. Thus, they enable assertions about whether celebrity endorsements outperform a single kind of endorsement or no endorsement. By contrast, meta-analysis enables comparisons across all types of endorsements simultaneously. Therefore, we can see whether celebrity endorsements outperform any other kind of endorsement. We can also test whether specific differences in performance (e.g., celebrity vs. expert) are significantly different from other performance differences (e.g., celebrity vs. ordinary consumer). Marketing managers can thus gain valuable knowledge when deciding on celebrity endorsers, any other kind of endorsement, or no endorsement at all. We consequently ask:

RQ1: Do celebrity endorsements differ in their effectiveness depending on the control group applied?

A concise summary of the existing knowledge on celebrity endorsement effects can be found in Table 1 . Looking at the main results, celebrity endorsements are shown to affect cognitive, affective, and conative outcomes. Furthermore, most studies have looked at endorsements of for-profit causes. Results frequently appear to be mixed. In addition, some studies show no effects at all. This meta-analysis will shed light on these mixed results by calculating an overall effect. In addition, mixed results can be clarified by adding potential moderators to the analysis. Furthermore, the meta-analysis will close gaps in the literature by investigating between-study differences that cannot be explored with single studies (e.g., endorser sex, endorser type, endorsement explicitness). Looking at the investigated outcomes, most studies have investigated affective reactions followed by cognitive and conative ones. Meta-analysis will provide insights about whether there are any differences in terms of effectiveness per outcome type.

Study retrieval

Literature search.

Studies were collected from three major databases (Business Source Premier, PsychINFO, Communication and Mass Media Complete). The search included all peer-reviewed articles written in English and published through April 2016. The databases were examined using the term celebrit* in combination with endors*, spokes*, or advert* in any available search field. The search resulted in 1025 articles. About 300 of them were quantitative studies, including content analyses, surveys, and experimental studies.

Inclusion criteria

These quantitative studies were narrowed down based on the impact of celebrity endorsements on endorsed objects. Three criteria had to be met. First, only experimental studies were included because only they enable causal assertions (Shaughnessy and Zechmeister 1997 ). The studies had to compare an experimental group to a control group. While the experimental group had to feature a celebrity endorsing an object, the control group had to include the same object, either non-endorsed or endorsed by a non-celebrity spokesperson. Studies that compared various types of celebrity endorsements but did not feature a non-celebrity control group were excluded (e.g., Ambroise et al. 2014 ; Kamins 1990 ). Second, the celebrities had to be actually existing celebrities, thus excluding studies that investigated the impact of fictitious and imagined celebrities, as their validity is arguably limited. Third, the studies also had to report effect measures related to the endorsed object, excluding studies that solely reported measures related to the endorser (e.g., Cho 2010 ) or the general acceptance of celebrity endorsements (e.g., Becker 2013 ). In addition, a measure was considered only if it was possible to obtain at least two effect sizes. Otherwise, the meta-analyzed effect size would equal the sole obtained effect size, rendering meta-analysis useless. This resulted in 15 eligible measures: attention to and interest in an ad, awareness of an endorsed object (recognition and recall), attitude toward an ad, attitude toward the endorsed object, perceived credibility of the ad and advertiser, meaning transfer (in the sense of transferring a celebrity’s meaning to a brand), evoked feelings, estimated price of a product, taste of a product, estimated information value of an ad, planning to inform oneself more about an endorsed object, perceived increase of knowledge, perceived risk when buying or using a product, brand choice, and behavioral intentions (intention to purchase or use an object, intention to volunteer, intention to support a charitable cause by spending time or money, and intention to share an endorsed object online; cf. Motyka et al. 2014 ). No limitations were placed regarding the endorsed object encompassing any kind of object, such as product, brand, organization, behavior, or charitable cause.

Based on these criteria, 44 manuscripts remained (the majority of the 300 quantitative studies were content analyses, surveys, or experimental studies comparing celebrities with celebrities). Eight of these (Chou 2014 ; Fireworker and Friedman 1977 ; Freiden 1984 ; Jain et al. 2011 ; Ross et al. 1984 ; Sanbonmatsu and Kardes 1988 ; Veer et al. 2010 ) had to be excluded, as they lacked appropriate statistical information to calculate effect sizes with the formulas suggested by Lipsey and Wilson ( 2001 ). Beforehand, all authors had been contacted and asked to provide missing statistical information if possible. According to Eisend ( 2009 ), about 18% exclusion is not uncommon in meta-analysis, and it matches other meta-analyses in marketing (Brown and Stayman 1992 ; Szymanski et al. 1995 ; Tellis 1988 ). The remaining 36 manuscripts yielded 46 independent studies, coming to 10,357 participants.

Meta-analytic procedures

Effect size calculation.

The standardized mean difference (d) was used as the effect size estimate according to the formulas provided by Lipsey and Wilson ( 2001 ). All available statistical information was incorporated (e.g., means, standard deviations, t- and F-statistics, and frequencies). Since this effect size estimate has been shown to be upwardly biased when calculated from small sample sizes (Lipsey and Wilson 2001 ), all estimates were corrected for sample size bias (Hedges 1981 ). Positive d - values indicated a stronger effect of a celebrity endorsement compared to a non-endorsed or non-celebrity endorsed message, whereas negative d - values indicated a stronger effect of the non-endorsed or non-celebrity endorsed message. In total, 367 effect sizes were obtained. The ratio of effect sizes (367) to the number of studies (46) is the rule rather than the exception when analyzing various dependent variables (Eisend 2006 , 2009 ; Szymanski et al. 1995 ).

Effect size integration and meta-analysis

Estimates were based on random-effects models. Fixed-effects models assume that all studies included in the meta-analysis are practically identical, having the same true effect size. In contrast, random-effects models assume differing true effect sizes varying, for instance, because of different participants or treatments. Specifically, true effect sizes are assumed to be distributed around some mean whereby the studies included in the analysis are assumed to represent a random sample (Borenstein et al. 2009 ). This model was much more realistic, as participants and study settings certainly differed across studies. In addition, results may be generalized because the investigated studies are treated as a random subset of a larger study population (Hedges and Vevea 1998 ). Several studies reported results that enabled obtaining more than one effect size per dependent variable. Performing a meta-analysis on these studies would violate the assumption of independence of effect sizes and assign more weight to the studies producing more than one effect size. Previous studies mostly ignored these problems, aggregated effect sizes into a single effect size (or chose only one effect size per study), or performed the so-called shifting the unit of analysis approach (Cheung 2014 ). This approach averages effect sizes within differing units depending on the current research question (e.g., study as a unit or study characteristics, such as gender of participants as a unit).

While ignoring these problems is clearly not satisfactory, the latter two approaches are rather broadly accepted (Borenstein et al. 2009 ; Cooper 2010 ). However, aggregating effect sizes or choosing only one effect size per study may strongly reduce the number of effect sizes, thus lowering the power of statistical tests. In addition, statistical information is lost, resulting in less precise estimates. The same deficits apply to shifting the unit of analysis (Cheung 2014 ). Researchers recently suggested treating meta-analysis as a multilevel model to address these drawbacks (e.g., Cheung 2014 ; Field 2015 ; Konstantopoulos 2011 ). The basic idea nests the effect sizes (first level) within the studies (second level; Konstantopoulos 2011 ). The resulting model then looks like Eqs. ( 1 ) and ( 2 ):

“Effect sizes (γ) in the ith study are predicted from the ‘true’ effect size for that study (λ i ) and some error (e i ) (note that the variance of e i is the sampling variance of that study). The true effect size for a study is made up of the average population effect (β 0 ) – which is the thing we usually want to estimate in meta-analysis – and some between-study error (u i ) (note that the variance of this between-study error is the heterogeneity of effect sizes across studies, which in traditional meta-analysis is denoted as τ 2 )” (Field 2015 , p. 18).

Writing the model in a single-level notation results in Eq. ( 3 ):

In this equation, it becomes evident that the variance of an observed effect size (γ i ) is decomposed into a sampling variance component (e i ) and the between-study error or random effect (u i ), as in traditional meta-analysis. However, since u i denotes a study-specific random effect of an ith study, the same random effect can be assigned to effect sizes stemming from the same study while effect sizes stemming from different studies receive different random effects (Konstantopoulos 2011 ; Viechtbauer 2015 ). Consequently, all effect sizes can be taken into account without aggregation and loss of information. The dependence or independence of the effect sizes is explicitly modeled by assigning the correct random effect. Furthermore, a third level may be introduced when estimating an overall effect size composed of effect sizes (first level) nested within different types of effect sizes, that is, dependent variables (second level), which are, in turn, nested within different studies (third level). The variance would then be decomposed into sampling variance, between-type of effect size variability, and between-study variability. This analysis is necessary for testing whether it makes sense to analyze different types of effect sizes separately (between-type of effect size variability) and whether it makes sense to analyze our moderators at all (between-study variability; Konstantopoulos 2011 ).

Following these recommendations, all analyses were carried out using the rma.mv() function of the R metafor package (Viechtbauer 2010 ). A maximum likelihood estimator, the typical method to estimate multilevel models, was applied (see Konstantopoulos 2011 ; van den Noortgate et al. 2014 ). Average effect sizes were estimated taking the random-effects perspective, and moderator analyses were performed applying the mixed-effects models (meta-regression). As the studies showed considerable variance in sample size and some studies produced multiple effect size estimates, effect sizes were weighted by sample size and the number of effect sizes per study. Specifically, effects sizes were weighted by the ratio of their study’s sample size to the number of effect sizes measuring the same dependent variable within the study (Eisend 2009 ). As a result, studies reporting only one effect size received the same weight as studies reporting multiple effect sizes if their sample size was equal.

The moderators can be grouped as endorser variables, endorsed object variables, and endorsement type of the comparison group. The variables were coded by two independent coders based on the information available in the manuscripts and complemented by the English Wikipedia pages of celebrities where necessary. Agreement was perfect except for endorser match, which yielded an acceptable Krippendorff’s alpha of .74 (Krippendorff 2004 ). Discrepancies were resolved by discussion after a review of the article.

Endorser variables

The endorser’s sex was coded as female (0) or male (1), according to the description of the study authors. Typical descriptions were male/female, Mr./Mrs., or he/she. If the manuscript provided no gender information, the endorser’s English Wikipedia page was consulted. The endorser type was coded as actor (0), model (1), athlete (2), musician (3), or TV host (4), according to the description of the authors. The authors typically described their celebrities by using one of the aforementioned professions. If the manuscript provided no related information, the endorser’s English Wikipedia page was consulted. If the Wikipedia page presented various professions, the first was chosen.

The endorser match was coded as incongruent (0) or congruent (1), according to the description of the authors. Frequently used descriptions were not matching/matching, not fitting/fitting, or being incongruent/congruent to an endorsed object. Furthermore, some studies reported pretests explicitly testing the congruence of the endorser and endorsed object. The moderator was then coded accordingly. If the authors used existing advertising, the endorser was coded as congruent because advertisers usually put considerable time and effort into finding a matching endorser (Erdogan 1999 ). The endorsement explicitness was coded as implicit (0) or explicit (1) according to the description of the authors. Following *Miller and Allen ( 2012 ), the endorsement was coded as implicit when the endorser and the endorsed object appeared merely as paired without the endorser explicitly announcing his or her endorsement (e.g., classical conditioning procedure or ad merely displaying object and celebrity). An explicit endorsement was coded if the endorser’s support for an object could be explicitly read or heard by the study participants (e.g., “I think XY is …” or “I love XY”). In addition, signatures were coded as explicit endorsements.

The endorsement frequency was coded continuously starting from one (1) and stretching—theoretically—infinitely, though the maximum number of endorsements was 10.

Endorsed object variables

The familiarity of the endorsed object was coded as unfamiliar (0) or familiar (1) according to the description of the authors. Familiarity refers to whether the endorsed object was known by the participants. Typical descriptions in the articles were unknown/known, fictitious, or having a strong reputation. Furthermore, some studies reported pretests assessing object familiarity. The moderator was then coded accordingly.

We coded whether the comparison group perceived the object as non-endorsed (0; appearing without any support) or as endorsed by a non-celebrity spokesperson or organization. The endorsed categories were expert (1), an employee of the selling company (2), an ordinary consumer (3), an unknown model or athlete (4), a quality seal or award (5), a government employee Footnote 1 (6), or an endorser brand from the same product category (7). The authors typically described the comparison groups by using one of the aforementioned category names.

Overall analysis

Testing first whether it makes sense to analyze different types of effect sizes separately and whether it makes sense to analyze the moderators at all, we calculated the three-level model (Konstantopoulos 2011 ). Effect sizes (first level) were nested within different types of effect sizes (second level), which were, in turn, nested within different studies (third level). We observed no significant overall effect of the celebrity endorsements on participants’ responses (d = .04, 95% CI (−.09, .17), ns). However, highly significant heterogeneity was found among effect sizes (Q (366) = 1095.77, p  < .001). This suggests that effect sizes vary considerably due to the type of effect size differences (second level) and/or study differences (third level). The I 2 statistic—the amount of total variability (sampling variance + heterogeneity) that can be attributed to the heterogeneity among the true effects (Higgins and Thompson 2002 )—provided more details. About half the total variability could be attributed to the between-study heterogeneity (I 2  = 51.53%, third level) and about 11% could be attributed to the between-type of effect size within study heterogeneity (I 2  = 11.45%, second level). Hence, it seemed reasonable to explain the between-study heterogeneity by moderator analysis and to examine the different types of effect sizes separately, given their heterogeneity.

Table 2 shows the meta-analytic results for the most frequently investigated dependent variables. Footnote 2 The first column presents the average effect size for a specific dependent variable. The second and third column display subgroup results of this average effect size. They differentiate studies that featured a comparison group receiving no endorsement (second column) from studies that featured a comparison group receiving an object endorsed by a non-celebrity spokesperson (third column). In addition, the third column specifies the particular types of endorsements received by the comparison groups. There were almost no average effects for any of the dependent variables. Only one significant effect size emerged. Celebrity endorsements positively affected consumers attitudes toward the endorsed object when celebrity endorsements were compared to a non-endorsed condition (d = .24; 95% CI (.04, .43), p  < .05). As opposed to this affective effect, there were neither cognitive nor conative effects on average.

Moderator analysis

Moderator analyses were conducted for the dependent variables of attitude toward the endorsed object, attitude toward the ad, and behavioral intention, all featuring a sufficient number of effect sizes to conduct the analyses (Higgins and Green 2011 ). Tests for heterogeneity revealed significant heterogeneity among all three types of effect sizes (attitude toward the endorsed object: Q (116) = 372.67, I 2  = 68.87%, p  < .001; attitude toward the ad: Q (44) = 200.81, I 2  = 78.11%, p  < .001; behavioral intention: Q (92) = 188.33, I 2  = 51.15%, p  < .001). As indicated by the I 2 statistic, the level of heterogeneity was medium to high, suggesting great between-study variability that may be explained by the moderators (Huedo-Medina et al. 2006 ).

Table 3 presents the results of the first meta-regression testing the influence of the comparison group’s endorsement on the effect size for attitude toward the object. The analysis was conducted only for this dependent variable. Only this measure allowed for the simultaneous analysis of almost all the moderator categories. The category of employee of the selling company was missing because no study included both attitude toward the endorsed object and employee of the selling company. The comparison group’s endorsement is a categorical variable, so the moderator was dummy coded with no endorsement (0) representing the reference category (Field 2013 ). As a result, the intercept’s coefficient (Table 3 ), being significant and positive, represents the average effect size of celebrity endorsements compared to a non-endorsed group. The remaining regression coefficients represent the change in this effect size when the comparison group features some kind of endorsement instead of no endorsement. As seen in Table 3 , the effect size decreased when the comparison group featured some other kind of endorsement, as indicated by all the regression coefficients being negative. However, the decrease appeared to be significant only when celebrity endorsements were compared to endorsements of an unknown model or athlete, quality seal or award, government employee, or endorser brand. Subtracting these decreases from the intercept (.24), it became evident that the respective effect size was then negative in all cases. A separate test for significance revealed these negative effect sizes to be (marginally) significant for a quality seal or award endorsement, and a endorser brand (unknown model: d = −.32, 95% CI (−.73, .08), ns ; quality seal: d = −.44, 95% CI (−.81, −.07), p  < .05; government employee: d = −.45, 95% CI (−1.01, .12), ns; endorser brand: d = −.58, 95% CI (−1.16, .01), p  = .05). In terms of the research question, it can thus be concluded that celebrity endorsements perform worse compared to quality seals, award endorsements, or endorser brands, but they perform better when compared to no endorsement.

Further meta-regressions were conducted to integrate all assumed moderators (Table 4 ). The moderators were entered hierarchically starting with comparison group’s endorsement (Model 1), followed by endorser variables (Model 2), and eventually complemented by an endorsed object variable (Model 3). For the moderator comparison group’s endorsement, the categories were merged into two groups (no endorsement (0) vs. non-celebrity endorsement (1), as in Table 2 . The remaining moderators were coded as displayed in the methods section. The endorser type was dummy coded with actor (0) representing the reference category. The results can be seen in Table 4 . As can be seen, the impact of celebrity endorsements decreased when compared to an endorsed comparison group instead of a non-endorsed one (Model 1, first column). Looking at the endorser variables (Model 2, first column), we see that male endorsers performed substantially better than female endorsers, supporting H4. Looking at endorser type, effect size decreased when an object was endorsed by a model, athlete, musician, or TV host instead of an actor (the reference category). However, only a model, musician, or TV host performed significantly less well. Hypothesis 5 was thus partially confirmed. The product match-up hypothesis (H6) was supported, indicated by enhanced effects for congruent endorsers when compared to incongruent ones. By contrast, the seventh hypothesis was rejected, as implicit endorsements performed substantially better that explicit ones. This is the opposite of what we predicted. Likewise, the eighth hypothesis was rejected as no impact was seen for endorsement frequency.

As seen at the bottom of Table 4 , R 2 indicates that more than 91% of the heterogeneity could be explained by both blocks of moderators. This explanation was significant, as indicated by the test of moderators (Q M  = 82.11, p  < .001). The test for residual heterogeneity indicated a significant amount of heterogeneity remaining, possibly explained by the last moderator (Q = 119.08, p  < .001). Indeed, familiarity affected effect size as assumed in H9 (Model 3 first column). Celebrity endorsement effects were stronger for unfamiliar objects when compared to familiar ones. For attitude toward the ad and behavioral intention, the results appeared quite similar. The only differences were that endorser match had no influence on attitude toward the ad and that the influence of endorser type did partially differ in both cases. The moderators were able to explain 100 % of the heterogeneity. The tests for residual heterogeneity were accordingly insignificant. The impact of familiarity and endorsement frequency could not be tested as all effect sizes pertained to unknown objects endorsed once.

The combined impact of all moderators can be seen in Table 5 . It displays the predicted d values and confidence intervals for an actor endorsing an unfamiliar object a single time compared to no endorsement of the same object at varying levels of the endorser’s sex, endorser match, and endorsement mode. Values were calculated with the predict() function (Viechtbauer 2010 ). The d values for other endorser or object types (e.g., model or familiar object) may be obtained by adding or subtracting the respective regression coefficient from Table 4 . Based on the results for attitude toward the endorsed object, the highest effect size can be expected for male actors, matching the object, and endorsing it implicitly (d = .90; 95% CI (.54, 1.25)). In contrast, the lowest effect size appears for female actors not matching an explicitly endorsed object (d = −.58; 95% CI (−1.02, −.13). It is important to note that female celebrities can likewise have a positive impact, for instance, endorsing a congruent object implicitly (d = .44; 95% CI (.20, .68)). Again, effect size patterns were similar for attitude toward the ad and behavioral intention.

Publication bias

Before starting the bias analysis, the effect sizes were aggregated within the studies. Publication bias analysis checks whether certain studies are more likely to be published than others. Publication bias was assessed applying funnel plots and Egger’s regression test for funnel plot asymmetry (Egger et al. 1997 ). Figure 2 displays the funnel plots for attitude toward the endorsed object, attitude toward the ad, and behavioral intention. The x-axis indicates the observed effect size whereas the y-axis displays the standard error as an indicator of sample size (Borenstein et al. 2009 ). The funnel plots showed no evidence of publication bias in terms of smaller studies missing at the bottom left corner (i.e., no evidence that smaller studies with minor effect sizes failed to be published). This is further confirmed by Egger’s regression tests being insignificant in all cases (attitude toward endorsed object: t(28) = 1.86, p  = .07; attitude toward ad: t(8) = .01, p  = .99; behavioral intentions: t(15) = 1.45, p  = .17).

Funnel plots of the studies in the meta-analysis for various dependent variables

Main findings and contributions

The main findings are summarized in Table 6 . The analysis revealed a zero overall effect of celebrity endorsements on consumers’ responses. Yet there were strong effects on some dependent measures and under some conditions. The main contribution of our study, therefore, lies in understanding the possible causes of this variability rather than focusing on a summary effect (Thompson and Sharp 1999 ). This fits nicely with the notion of Lipsey and Wilson ( 2001 ) saying that “contemporary meta-analysis is increasingly attending to the variance of effect size distributions rather than the means of those distributions. That is, the primary question of interest often has to do with identifying the sources of differences in study findings” (p. 8f).

Different dependent measures

Surprisingly, almost no average effects were observed for standard measures, such as awareness, attitude toward the ad, or purchase intention. A significant and positive average effect size emerged only for attitude toward the endorsed object. Furthermore, this was the case only when the respective comparison group was not endorsed. This is consistent with the meta-regression results assessing the influence of comparison group’s endorsement type on attitude toward the advertised object (Table 3 ). Celebrity endorsements positively affected consumers’ attitudes compared to no endorsement, and this effect was significantly lower and negative when celebrity endorsements were compared to an unknown model or athlete, a quality seal or award, a government employee, or an endorser brand. Hence, several low or zero average effect sizes were at least partially due to being based on comparison groups producing negative effect sizes and on comparison groups producing positive effect sizes. As a result, there were small or no effects on average. Interestingly, although effect sizes lowered significantly in the aforementioned cases (unknown model or athlete, quality seal or award, government employee, endorser brand), celebrity endorsements performed worse only when compared to a quality seal or award, or an endorser brand. In these cases, the effect sizes were negative and significant themselves rather than just significantly lower (Viechtbauer 2010 ).

We integrated several endorser variables and the properties of the endorsed object. Overall, the moderators appeared very effective in explaining the between-study heterogeneity accounting for 95–100% of the variability with respect to three key dependent variables (attitude toward the endorsed object, attitude toward the ad, behavioral intention). Moreover, all moderated effects came into existence when controlling for the other moderators (Field 2013 ). For the endorser variables, the impact of the endorsement frequency on effect size was investigated. Contrary to our assumption, there was no effect. This result contradicts conventional thinking that endorsements’ effectiveness is enhanced with increasing repetitions (*Till et al. 2008 ). Although endorsement effects have been found at single exposures (Ambroise et al. 2014 ), advertisements are assumed to be learned more thoroughly when exposed multiple times (wearin), as long as consumers are not bored or annoyed by too much repetition (wearout; Campbell and Keller 2003 ). Wearout seems rather unlikely in this study, as the maximum number of exposures included in the specific analysis was five, and as “five pairings is not so many as to cause subject boredom but is likely to lead to conditioning effects” (*Till et al. 2008 ). With the maximum number of five repetitions, the amount of variance needed to produce an effect of repetition was most likely too small. For instance, Stuart et al. ( 1987 ) included up to 20 repetitions in their conditioning study to test for frequency effects. In addition, it was impossible to control for exposure time since most studies failed to report it. Hence, some studies might have exposed their participants once but for a rather long time, while others may have exposed their participants multiple times in short durations. Both manipulations, repetition and duration, may have resulted in similar effects. Given these limitations, it seems premature to reject the frequency hypothesis. Instead, future studies should account for exposure time, too. These studies should also look for a possible suppression effect (Koch and Zerback 2013 ). Specifically, repeated celebrity endorsements may lead to enhanced advertising outcomes while at the same increasing the likelihood of evoking reactance due to too much (forced) exposure. As result, increased reactance may reflect negatively on advertising outcomes and suppress the positive effect of endorsement repetition. That is, the effect of repetition on advertising outcomes may be positive and negative (mediated through reactance) at the same time leading to a zero total effect.

For endorser type, actors performed best followed by athletes and TV hosts, which were followed by models and musicians. The results were quite similar across the three dependent variables. As initially outlined, the enhanced effects of actors may best be explained by consumers being exposed to them audiovisually as well as multiple times over the years. As a result, consumers are likely to develop stronger consumer–celebrity relationships (Klimmt et al. 2006 ). This is particularly true with a TV series (Hoffner and Buchanan 2005 ). In addition, actors may generally be more famous, at least compared to models, which explains the comparatively weak effects of the latter.

Relatively strong effects appeared regarding the endorser’s sex. Male celebrities evoked substantially stronger effects compared to female celebrities. We attribute the stronger effects to the male spokespersons’ greater prestige and expertise resulting in stronger credibility (Kenton 1989 ; Whittaker 1965 ). Compared to all previous research (see Erdogan 1999 ), we are now able to provide practitioners with a clear effect direction. In addition to the sex differences, this study is the first to confirm the product match-up hypothesis on a meta-analytic level. Congruent endorsers produce significantly greater effect sizes compared to incongruent ones. Interestingly, the hypothesis could only be confirmed with regard to attitude toward the endorsed object and behavioral intention. Both refer to evaluations or behavior directly related to the endorsed object. In contrast, no effect was found for attitude toward the ad. Hence, celebrity–object match matters when it comes to attitudes toward the advertised object and purchasing the object. However, celebrity–object match does not necessarily matter when it comes to attitudes toward the ad. This is due to the fact that an object does not necessarily equal the style or imagery of its ad. Both may be completely different. Just think about a company intending to change the image of its brand. Employed ads will likely be quite different from the existing brand image in order to change the image. Hence, a match with an endorsed object is evidently of less importance when evaluating the ad because object and ad do not necessarily equal. In addition, marketers and researchers usually test the match with an endorsed object and not the match with the advertisement itself (e.g., *Chen et al. 2012 ; Kamins 1990 ; *Kamins and Gupta 1994 ). Effects may arise when explicitly manipulating the match with an ad.

For endorsement explicitness, strong effects appeared, yet opposite the direction we expected. Celebrities implicitly endorsing an object enhanced consumers’ attitudes and behavioral intentions substantially more compared to explicit endorsements. The Persuasion Knowledge Model presents a possible explanation (Friestad and Wright 1994 ). According to this model, consumers develop persuasion knowledge throughout a lifetime of being exposed to persuasive communication. This knowledge is likely to be activated when consumers recognize a persuasive attempt. For instance, consumers tend to recognize persuasive attempts when messages “create the perception that the source was trying ‘too hard’ to sell his case” (Smith 1977 , p. 198). Explicit endorsements then act as some kind of forewarning of the persuasive intent of the endorser. Consumers are more motivated to counterargue the endorsement in order to reassert their freedom (Petty and Cacioppo 1979 ). This is particularly likely if the endorsed object is of high relevance (Petty and Cacioppo 1979 ).

Another explanation for this counterintuitive finding may be the fact that implicit endorsement are mostly perceived as merely conveying a celebrity’s personal object experience (e.g., by using a brand), whereas explicit endorsements are perceived as conveying a clear recommendation to buy or use an object. As a result, implicit endorsements try to persuade their audience to a lesser extent compared to explicit ones. According to Pornpitakpan ( 2004 ), this should enhance endorsers’ trustworthiness, leading to stronger effects (see Pornpitakpan 2004 ). Future studies should test whether implicit endorsers are perceived as more trustworthy and less likely to activate persuasion knowledge.

In terms of familiarity, celebrity endorsements appeared more effective in the case of unfamiliar objects when compared to familiar ones. This result was expected and is in line with past research. Given that consumers already possess a rich network of associations representing an object, attitudes and behavior appear more difficult to change (Cacioppo et al. 1992 ). That is not to say that outcomes related to familiar objects cannot be influenced at all. The influence is just weaker or more difficult to accomplish.

Theoretical and managerial implications

The hierarchy of advertising effects model by Lavidge and Steiner ( 1961 ), which was adapted from Grewal et al. ( 1997 ), provided a fruitful theoretical framework. It enabled a systematic organization of all relevant advertising outcomes as well as the integration of relevant moderators (Lipsey and Wilson 2001 ). In addition, readers can grasp at a glance which relationships have been tested and which relationships have been impossible to test, although they might be theoretically relevant. Future studies can precisely look at these relationships. Furthermore, the model—being a broad overarching framework—enabled the integration of various sub-theories to explain specific effects.

The match-up hypothesis could be supported on a meta-analytic level. The proposed congruency effect can be regarded as quite robust. However, it was not possible to test which of the theoretical explanations is more accurate. In fact, both may be accurate: a matching celebrity being an information source of adaptive significance (Social Adaptation Theory) is also more likely to be easily integrated with existing brand schemas (Schema Theory). Instead of continuing to prove the effect, future research should rather look for its underlying psychological mechanism as well as boundary conditions. It still remains difficult to know which dimensions should be matched between an endorsed object and a celebrity (Amos et al. 2008 ).

The meta-analysis was also able to support the familiarity proposition. It suggests that attitudes toward unfamiliar objects are easier to change when compared to familiar objects (Cacioppo et al. 1992 ). That is due to the fact that attitudes are based on attitude-relevant information. If relatively little attitude-relevant information is available in memory, attitudes are primarily based on the information provided by the celebrity endorsement, leading to stronger effects. By contrast, if consumers have access to a relatively large body of attitude-relevant information in memory, attitudes are based on the endorsement and information in memory. As a result, endorsement effects are comparably smaller (Cacioppo et al. 1992 ). Since ads typically feature familiar objects, future research is advised to look for factors boosting celebrity endorsement effects in the case of high familiarity (Kent and Allen 1994 ). Endorsement repetition or a particular strong consumer–celebrity relationship may present such factors.

In terms of managerial implications, marketers should consider the following findings when choosing their endorser. The findings are accompanied by an assessment of their impact magnitude. Magnitude was assessed according to an effect size investigation from 300 meta-analyses (d < .30: small: d = .50: medium; d > .67: large; Lipsey and Wilson 2001 ), providing marketers with guidance when deciding on which of the findings to focus more.

Matching endorsers elicit more favorable attitudes and stronger behavioral intentions when compared to non-matching ones (impact magnitude: medium).

Male endorsers elicit more favorable attitudes and stronger behavioral intentions when compared to female ones (impact magnitude: medium to large).

Implicit endorsements elicit more favorable attitudes and stronger behavioral intentions when compared to explicit ones (impact magnitude: medium to large).

Actors elicit more favorable attitudes when compared to models, musicians, and TV hosts (impact magnitude: small to medium).

Endorsements of unfamiliar objects elicit more favorable attitudes when compared to endorsements of familiar objects (impact magnitude: small).

Celebrity endorsements elicit less favorable attitudes when compared to endorsements by quality seals, awards, and endorser brands (impact magnitude: medium).

In general, celebrity endorsements are undoubtedly an effective way of marketing communication. They enhance attitudes and reinforce behavioral intentions, provided marketers choose the right endorser. Marketers have to make these choices carefully, as celebrity endorsements can evoke strong negative outcomes, too. Incongruent male or female celebrities may very likely result in negative effects when endorsing an object explicitly. Marketers are, therefore, advised to back these decisions with market research (Agrawal and Kamakura 1995 ).

Limitations and agenda for future research

A great advantage of meta-analysis lies in identifying significant gaps in the literature. Such gaps become visible when the meta-analysis cannot report findings on a particular dependent variable or topic. In addition, a meta-analysis may leave blind spots because theoretically relevant moderators cannot be analyzed, as too few studies have investigated these. We discuss these gaps in the following sections.

Understudied dependent variables

The most important understudied variables pertain to recognition and recall, meaning transfer, and behavioral measures in general. Beginning with recognition and recall, only about ten effect sizes could be obtained from the literature regarding each measure. This deficit is particularly relevant as marketers are frequently interested in favorable object recognition and recall employing celebrity endorsements (Erfgen et al. 2015 ). Moreover, the revealed average effect was close to zero (cf. awareness Table 2 ). This suggests no impact or a moderated impact of celebrity endorsements. Having robust knowledge of one or the other is vital to marketers. “A common concern is that consumers will focus their attention on the celebrity and fail to notice the brand being promoted” (Erdogan 1999 , p. 296). Researchers have recently started to investigate this concern, concluding that celebrities might indeed overshadow an endorsed object (Erfgen et al. 2015 ).

A similar pattern appears when looking at meaning transfer. Marketers frequently seek to transfer celebrity meaning to a brand to build or reposition its image (Keller 2012 ). However, research investigating celebrity meaning transfer is relatively scarce. It only started a few years ago, resulting in a limited number of effect sizes that could be obtained (k = 9; Galli and Gorn 2011 ). Though the existing studies suggest strong transfer effects, further research is needed, also specifying boundary conditions (e.g., brand familiarity, *Miller and Allen 2012 ). Finally, researchers should dedicate more resources to measuring behavior. As seen in Table 2 , scholars only measured behavioral intentions. While closely related, behavioral intentions do not fully explain true behavior (Kim and Hunter 1993 ).

Understudied moderators

For the theoretically relevant moderators, our study could not integrate several moderators due to missing coding information, no variability within the moderators, or simply a lack of studies. The advertising vehicles in which the celebrity endorsements were integrated present such a factor (cf. Table 1 ). Almost all the studies employed print advertisements or similar stimuli. In contrast, very few studies looked at radio, television, or online advertising, rendering moderator analysis impossible (e.g., *Myrick and Evans 2014 ; *Toncar et al. 2007 ; *Wei and Lu 2013 ). This seems even more serious considering that more than 60% of global advertising can be attributed to television and the Internet (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ). Also, exposure time could not be included. Increasing exposure time enhances processing capacity, which may lead to stronger and more durable effects (Petty and Cacioppo 1986 ). Similarly, few studies provided information about the endorser’s valence (positive vs. negative), trustworthiness, attractiveness, or expertise.

Longer-term effects

Thus far, almost all studies measured effects immediately after exposure. This is particularly problematic as advertisers are mostly interested in longer-term effects (Eisend and Langner 2010 ). In addition, various advertising studies have shown that the effectiveness of advertisements varies across time (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ). For instance, Eisend and Langner ( 2010 ) were able to show that a celebrity’s attractiveness exerts its main impact right after exposure while expertise exhibits its main influence in a delayed situation. They conclude that “effects on attitude toward the brand can considerably differ depending on whether the measurement occurs immediately after ad exposure or with a delay” and that studies would strongly “benefit from including delayed measures in ad testing, particularly when they deal with celebrity endorsers in the advertisements” (Eisend and Langner 2010 , p. 543).

Underlying psychological processes

In addition, hardly any study looked at the underlying psychological mechanisms of celebrity persuasion (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ). Instead, research was mostly focused on showing main effects or on looking for possible moderators. As a result, theoretical depth is rather low (Bergkvist and Zhou 2016 ). For instance, there is no clear knowledge about whether endorsement effects vary depending on high or low effort processing (Petty and Cacioppo 1986 ). The few studies that integrated processing style suggested stronger effects in low effort processing (*Dong 2015 ; *Petty et al. 1983 ; *Sengupta et al. 1997 ). However, several studies investigating other issues in celebrity endorsements found strong effects when participants fully concentrated on the endorsements (e.g., *Friedman and Friedman 1979 ; *Kamins and Gupta 1994 ; *La Ferle and Choi 2005 ). Further research is needed that clarifies this matter and looks for other possible mechanisms besides message elaboration.

Non-profit advertising

Non-profit advertising certainly deserves more attention because it has been steadily growing over the past years including politics, the health sector, or any kind of NGO communication (*Wheeler 2009 ). Yet research is still very limited. For instance, whether celebrity endorsements can change voting behavior is still understudied (*Pease and Brewer 2008 ). Accordingly, van Steenburg ( 2015 ) recently concluded a review by asking: “Are voters consumers? Can the two be treated similarly when it comes to marketing strategy and the marketing mix? Is selling a candidate the same as selling a car? Do theoretical foundations of consumer behaviour hold in voter behaviour? […] As of yet, all of these represent untapped discoveries” (p. 216). The same applies to the effectiveness of celebrity endorsements in any kind of health or health-related communication as well as environmental communication (Boyland et al. 2013 ; *Myrick and Evans 2014 ; *Wu et al. 2012 ).

Cross-cultural differences

So far, the majority of studies has been conducted in the U.S. However, there is undoubtedly a strong interest in celebrity endorsements in such emerging countries as India or China (Chou 2014 ; Mishra and Mishra 2014 ). And even though more and more studies are conducted in Asia, it has yet to be tested whether the same mechanisms that apply to Western celebrity endorsements apply to Asian cultures as well. Because a lot of aspects of consumer behavior are culture-bound, culture-adequate methods are urgently needed (de Mooij and Hofstede 2011 ).

Side effects

Finally, future research may also dedicate itself more strongly to side effects or unintended effects. This pertains particularly to vulnerable audiences like, for instance, children or adolescents. It is widely accepted that childhood and adolescence is the developmental period during which human beings complete the process of identity formation (Lloyd 2002 ). Next to their family and friends, they frequently refer to mass media when looking for role models (Hoffner and Buchanan 2005 ). Celebrities depicted in the media and advertising can serve as such models given that they are considered relevant by consumers (Lockwood and Kunda 1997 ). This may pose a problem because children’s and adolescents’ understanding of advertising may not be as advanced when compared to adults. Specifically, past research has revealed that children do not necessarily comprehend the persuasive intent of advertising and constitute a vulnerable audience, deserving of special protection (Kunkel 2001 ).

The study sought to quantify the effectiveness of celebrity endorsements on a meta-analytic level across a variety of measures. The results showed a zero effect when averaging across all studies. However, we found strong attitudinal and behavioral effects when including theoretically relevant moderator variables. In particular, effects on attitudes and behavior were found to be strongest when choosing a male actor that matches the endorsed object and expresses his endorsement implicitly. Given the continuing growth of celebrity endorsements in product marketing, politics, and health communication, the study provides essential knowledge to researchers and marketers.

Summary of the meta-analysis by Amos et al. ( 2008 )

This meta-analysis focused on source effects of celebrity endorsers on advertising effectiveness. Analyzed source variables were negative information, expertise, attractiveness, credibility, trustworthiness, likeability, familiarity, and performance. Advertising effectiveness was understood rather broadly, combining various measures of effectiveness into one effect size variable (purchase intention, brand attitude, attitude toward advertisement, believability, recall, and recognition). The actual analysis focused on the comparison of effect sizes according to the source variables. In addition, it was tested whether effect sizes significantly differ according to four methodological dimensions: surveys vs. experiments; student vs. non-student samples; U.S. vs. non-U.S. studies; main vs. interaction effects. Thirty two studies published through 2004 were part of the analysis. It included surveys and experiments whereby the majority of effect sizes were obtained from surveys.

Compared to the present analysis, Amos et al. ( 2008 ) did not test whether the obtained effect sizes were significant, but solely whether they were significant different from each other (according to various source variables). Hence, no results were provided that enable the assertion that celebrity endorsements exert a significant influence as well as an assertion about its size. In addition, there were no results for individual measures of advertising effectiveness (e.g., cognitive, affective, conative) because all measures were combined into one variable. Furthermore, Amos et al. ( 2008 ) neither accounted for dependency among the obtained effect sizes nor provided results in terms of the performance of celebrity endorsements when compared to other kinds of endorsements. Last but not least, results are less clear in terms of a causal interpretation since surveys were included predominantly. We want to point out that we do not perceive our analysis as more valuable, but rather as focusing on completely different aspects as Amos et al. ( 2008 ).

Endorsement by a government employee refers to a study by Frizzell ( 2011 ) in which the effectiveness of a celebrity endorsement was compared against the endorsement of a State Department representative (political issue).

Meta-analytic results for less frequently investigated dependent variables (k < 10) can be obtained from the authors upon request.

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Agrawal, J., & Kamakura, W. A. (1995). The economic worth of celebrity endorsers: an event study analysis. Journal of Marketing, 59 (3), 56–62.

Article   Google Scholar  

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50 (2), 179–211.

Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (1987). Dimensions of consumer expertise. Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (4), 411–454.

Ambroise, L., Pantin-Sohier, G., Valette-Florence, P., & Albert, N. (2014). From endorsement to celebrity co-branding: personality transfer. Journal of Brand Management, 21 (4), 273–285.

Amos, C., Holmes, G., & Strutton, D. (2008). Exploring the relationship between celebrity endorser effects and advertising effectiveness. A quantitative synthesis of effect size. International Journal of Advertising, 27 (2), 209–234.

Google Scholar  

Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40 (4), 471–499.

*Aureliano-Silva, L., Lopes, E. L. de Lamonica Freire, O. B., & da Silva, D. (2016). The brand’s effect on the evaluation of advertising endorsed by celebrities: an experimental study. Brazilian Business Review, 12 (4), 57–78.

Bargh, J. A., & Pratto, F. (1986). Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22 (4), 293–311.

Becker, A. B. (2013). Star power? Advocacy, receptivity, and viewpoints on celebrity involvement in issue politics. Atlantic . Journal of Communication, 21 (1), 1–16.

Bergkvist, L., & Zhou, K. Q. (2016). Celebrity endorsements: a literature review and research agenda. International Journal of Advertising, 35 , 642–663.

*Biswas, D., Biswas, A., & Das, N. (2006). The differential effect of celebrity and expert endorsements on consumer risk perceptions. The role of consumer knowledge, perceived congruency, and product technology orientation. Journal of Advertising, 35 (2), 17–31.

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Statistics in practice . Chichester, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons.

Book   Google Scholar  

Boyland, E. J., Harrold, J. A., Dovey, T. M., Allison, M., Dobson, S., Jacobs, M. C., & Halford, J. C. G. (2013). Food choice and overconsumption: effect of a premium sports celebrity endorser. The Journal of Pediatrics, 163 (2), 339–343.

Brown, S. P., & Stayman, D. M. (1992). Antecedents and consequences of attitude toward the ad: a meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (1), 34–51.

Cabalero, M., Lumpkin, J. R., & Madden, C. S. (1989). Using physical attractiveness as an advertising tool: An empirical test of attraction phenomenon. Journal of Advertising, 29 (4), 16–22.

Cacioppo, J. T., Marshall-Goodell, B. S., Tassinary, L. G., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: classical conditioning is more effective when prior knowledge about the attitude stimulus is low than high. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28 (3), 207–233.

Campbell, M. C., & Keller, K. L. (2003). Brand familiarity and advertising repetition effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (2), 292–304.

*Chao, P., Wührer, G., & Werani, T. (2005). Celebrity and foreign brand name as moderators of country-of-origin effects. International Journal of Advertising, 24 (2), 173–192.

*Chen, C. Y., Lin, Y. H., & Hsiao, C. L. (2012). Celebrity endorsement for sporting events using classical conditioning. International Journal of Sports Marketing & Sponsorship, 13 (3), 209–219.

Cheung, M. W.-L. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: a structural equation modeling approach. Psychological Methods, 19 (2), 211–229.

Cho, V. (2010). The endorser’s persuasiveness on the purchase intention of high-involvement products: a comparison between a newly launched product and a mature one. Journal of Global Marketing, 23 (3), 226–242.

Chou, H. Y. (2014). Effects of endorser types in political endorsement advertising. International Journal of Advertising, 33 (2), 391–414.

Chou, H. Y. (2015). Celebrity political endorsement effects: a perspective on the social distance of political parties. International Journal of Communication, 9 , 523–546.

Cooper, H. M. (2010). Research synthesis and meta-analysis. a step-by-step approach. Applied social research methods series (Vol. Vol. 2). Los Angeles: Sage.

Corbett, J. B., & Mori, M. (1999). Medicine, media, and celebrities: news coverage of breast cancer, 1960–1995. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 76 (2), 229–249.

Crutchfield, D. (2010). Celebrity endorsements still push product. Why, in the era of social media, the rewards continue to outweigh the risks. Advertising Age , (accessed September 8, 2015), [available at http://adage.com/article/cmo-strategy/marketing-celebrity-endorsements-push-product/146023 ].

De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37 (1), 1–20.

De Mooij, M., & Hofstede, G. (2011). Cross-cultural consumer behavior: a review of research findings. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 23 (3), 181–192.

*Dean, D. H., & Biswas, A. (2001). Third-party organization endorsement of products: an advertising cue affecting consumer prepurchase evaluation of goods and services. Journal of Advertising, 30 (4), 41–57.

Dibble, J. L., Hartmann, T., & Rosaen, S. F. (2016). Parasocial interaction and parasocial relationship: conceptual clarification and a critical assessment of measure. Human Communication Research, 42 (1), 21–44.

*Dixon, H., Scully, M., Kelly, B., Donovan, R., Chapman, K., & Wakefield, M. (2014). Counter-advertising may reduce parent’s susceptibility to front-of-package promotions on unhealthy foods. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 46 (6), 467–474.

*Dong, Z. (2015). How to persuade adolescents to use nutrition labels: effects of health consciousness, argument quality, and source credibility. Asian Journal of Communication, 25 (1), 84–101.

Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315 (7109), 629–634.

Eisend, M. (2006). Two-sided advertising: a meta-analysis. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 23 (2), 187–198.

Eisend, M. (2009). A meta-analysis of humor in advertising. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 37 (2), 191–203.

Eisend, M., & Langner, T. (2010). Immediate and delayed advertising effects of celebrity endorsers’ attractiveness and competence fit. International Journal of Advertising, 29 ( 4 ), 527–546.

Elberse, A., & Verleun, J. (2012). The economic value of celebrity endorsements. Journal of Advertising Research, 52 (2), 149–165.

Elliot, R., & Wattanasuwan, K. (1998). Brands as symbolic resources for the construction of identity. International Journal of Advertising, 17 (2), 131–144.

Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: a literature review. Journal of Marketing Management, 15 (4), 291–314.

*Erfgen, C., Zenker, S., & Sattler, H. (2015). The vampire effect: when do celebrity endorsers harm brand recall? International Journal of Research in Marketing, 32 (2), 155–163.

Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. And sex and drugs and rock ‘n’ roll . Los Angeles: SAGE.

Field, A. (2015). Dread returns to mega-silly one. Health Psychology Review, 9 (1), 15–20.

Fireworker, R. B., & Friedman, H. H. (1977). The effects of endorsements on product evaluation. Decision Sciences, 8 (3), 576–583.

Fleck, N., Korchia, M., & Roy, I. L. (2012). Celebrities in advertising: looking for congruence or likability. Psychology and Marketing, 29 (9), 651–662.

*Freiden, J. B. (1982). An evaluation of spokesperson and vehicle source effects in advertising. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 5 (1), 77–88.

Freiden, J. B. (1984). Advertising spokesperson effects: an examination of endorser type and gender on two audiences. Journal of Advertising Research, 24 (5), 33–41.

*Friedman, H. H., & Friedman, L. (1979). Endorser effectiveness by product type. Journal of Advertising Research, 19 (5), 63–71.

*Friedman, H. H., Termini, S., & Washington, R. (1976). The effectiveness of advertisements utilizing four types of endorsers. Journal of Advertising, 5 (3), 22–24.

Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model: how people cope with persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (1), 1–31.

*Frizzell, C. (2011). Public opinion and foreign policy: the effects of celebrity endorsements. The Social Science Journal, 48 (2), 314–323.

Galli, M., & Gorn, G. (2011). Unconscious transfer of meaning to brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21 (3), 215–225.

Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: a classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46 (1), 94–101.

Grewal, D., Kavanoor, S., Fern, E. F., Costley, C., & Barnes, J. (1997). Comparative versus noncomparative advertising: a meta-analysis. Journal of Marketing, 61 (4), 1–15.

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6 (2), 107–128.

Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3 (4), 486–504.

Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. Journal of Psychology, 21 (1), 107–112.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations . New York: Wiley and Sons.

Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1.0. (accessed September 8, 2015), [available at www.cochrane-handbook.org ].

Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21 (11), 1539–1558.

Hoffner, C., & Buchanan, M. (2005). Young adults’ wishful identification with television characters: the role of perceived similarity and character attributes. Media Psychology, 7 (4), 325–351.

Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Marín-Martínez, F., & Botella, J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychological Methods, 11 (2), 193–206.

*Jackson, D. J. (2008). Selling politics. Journal of Political Marketing, 6 (4), 67–83.

*Jackson, D. J., & Darrow, T. I. A. (2005). The influence of celebrity endorsements on young adults’ political opinions. The Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 10 (3), 80–98.

Jain, V., Roy, S., Daswani, A., & Sudha, M. (2011). What really works for teenagers: human or fictional celebrity? Young Consumers, 12 (2), 171–183.

Kahle, L. R., & Homer, P. M. (1985). Physical attractiveness of the celebrity endorser: a social adaptation perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (4), 954–961.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kaikati, J. G. (1987). Celebrity advertising: a review and synthesis. International Journal of Advertising, 6 (2), 93–105.

*Kamins, M. A. (1989). Celebrity and noncelebrity advertising in a two-sided context. Journal of Advertising Research, 29 (3), 34–43.

Kamins, M. A. (1990). An investigation into the “match-up” hypothesis in celebrity advertising: when beauty may be only skin deep. Journal of Advertising, 19 (1), 4–13.

*Kamins, M. A., & Gupta, K. (1994). Congruence between spokesperson and product type: a matchup hypothesis perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 11 (6), 569–586.

Keller, K. L. (2012). Strategic brand management. Building, measuring, and managing brand equity . Boston: Pearson.

Kent, R. J., & Allen, C. T. (1994). Competitive interference effects in consumer memory for advertising: the role of brand familiarity. Journal of Marketing, 58 (3), 97–105.

Kenton, S. B. (1989). Speaker credibility in persuasive business communication: a model which explains gender differences. Journal of Business Communication, 26 (2), 143–157.

Kilburn, D. (1998). Star power. Adweek, 39 (2), 20–21.

Kim, M. S., & Hunter, J. E. (1993). Attitude-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of attitudinal relevance and topic. Journal of Communication, 43 (1), 101–142.

Klimmt, C., Hartmann, T., & Schramm, H. (2006). Parasocial interactions and relationships. In J. Bryant & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of entertainment (pp. S291–S313). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Koch, T., & Zerback, T. (2013). Helpful or harmful? How frequent repetition affects perceived statement credibility. Journal of Communication, 63 (6), 993–1010.

Konstantopoulos, S. (2011). Fixed effects and variance components estimation in three-level meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 2 (1), 61–76.

Krippendorff, K. (2004). Content analysis: an introduction to its methodology . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kunkel, D. (2001). Children and television advertising. In D. G. Singer & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of children and the media (pp. 375–393). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

*La Ferle, C., & Choi, S. M. (2005). The importance of perceived endorser credibility in South Korean advertising. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 27 (2), 67–81.

Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication, 50 (1), 46–70.

Lavidge, R. J., & Steiner, G. A. (1961). A model for predictive measurements of advertising effectiveness. Journal of Marketing, 25 (6), 59–62.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Applied social research methods series (Vol. 49). SAGE: Thousand Oaks.

Lloyd, B. T. (2002). A conceptual framework for examining adolescent identity, media influence, and social development. Review of General Psychology, 6 (1), 73–91.

Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: predicting the impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (1), 91–103.

Lynch, J., & Schuler, D. (1994). The matchup effect of spokesperson and product congruency: a schema theory interpretation. Psychology and Marketing, 11 (5), 417–445.

*Maronick, T. J. (2005). Celebrity versus company president as endorsers of high risk products for elderly consumers. Journal of Promotion Management, 11 (4), 63–80.

*Martín-Santana, J. D., & Beerli-Palacio, A. (2013). Magazine advertising: factors influencing the effectiveness of celebrity advertising. Journal of Promotion Management, 19 (2), 139–166.

McCracken, G. (1989). Who is the celebrity endorser? Cultural foundations of the endorsement process. Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (3), 310–321.

*Miller, F. M., & Allen, C. T. (2012). How does celebrity meaning transfer? Investigating the process of meaning transfer with celebrity affiliates and mature brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22 (3), 443–452.

Mishra, A. A., & Mishra, A. (2014). National vs. local celebrity endorsement and politics. International Journal of Politics, Culture, and Society, 27 (4), 409–425.

Motyka, S., Grewal, D., Puccinelli, N. M., Roggeveen, A. L., Avnet, T., Daryanto, A., de Ruyter, K., & Wetzels, M. (2014). Regulatory fit: a meta-analytic synthesis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24 (3), 394–410.

Mowen, J. C., & Brown, S. W. (1981). On explaining and predicting the effectiveness of celebrity endorsers. In K. B. Monroe (ed), Advances in consumer research (pp. 437–441). Ann Abor, MI: Association for Consumer Research.

*Myrick, J. G., & Evans, S. D. (2014). Do PSAs take a bite out of shark week? The effects of juxtaposing environmental messages with violent images of shark attacks. Science Communication, 36 (5), 544–569.

*Park, M., Turner, B. A., & Pastore, D. L. (2008). Effective public service advertisements to attract volunteers for the Special Olympics: an elaboration likelihood perspective. Sport Management Review, 11 , 165–192.

*Pease, A., & Brewer, P. R. (2008). The Oprah factor: the effects of a celebrity endorsement in a presidential primary campaign. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 13 (4), 386–400.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1979). Effects of forwarning of persuasive intent and involvement on cognitive responses and persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology, 5 (2), 173–176.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19 , 123–205.

*Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: the moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (2), 135–146.

Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: a critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34 (2), 243–281.

Pringle, H., & Binet, L. (2005). How marketers can use celebrities to sell more effectively. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 4 (3), 201–214.

*Roozen, I., & Claeys, C. (2010). The relative effectiveness of celebrity endorsement for print advertisement. Review of Business and Economics, 1 , 76–89.

Ross, R. P., Campbell, T., Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Rice, M. L., & Turk, P. (1984). When celebrities talk, children listen: an experimental analysis of children’s responses to TV ads with celebrity endorsement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 5 (3), 185–202.

*Roy, S. (2012). To use the obvious choice: investigating the relative effectiveness of an overexposed celebrity. Journal of Research for Consumers, 22 , 41–69.

Russell, C. A., & Stern, B. B. (2006). Consumers, characters, and products: a balance model of sitcom product placement effects. Journal of Advertising, 35 (1), 7–21.

Sanbonmatsu, D. M., & Kardes, F. R. (1988). The effects of physiological arousal on information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (3), 379–385.

*Sengupta, J., Goodstein, R. C., & Boninger, D. S. (1997). All cues are not created equal: obtaining attitude persistence under low- involvement conditions. Journal of Consumer Research, 23 (4), 351–361.

Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1997). Research methods in psychology . New York: McGraw-Hill.

Shimp, T. A., Stuart, E. W., & Engle, R. W. (1991). A program of classical conditioning Experiments testing variations in the conditioned stimulus and context. Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (1), 1–12.

*Siemens, J. C., Smith, S., Fisher, D., & Jensen, T. D. (2008). Product expertise versus professional expertise: Congruency between an endorser’s chosen profession and the endorsed product. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing , 16 (3), 159–168.

Smith, M. J. (1977). The effects of threats to attitudinal freedom as a function of message quality and initial receiver attitude. Communication Monographs, 44 (August), 196–206.

*Stafford, M. R., Stafford, T. F., & Day, E. (2002). A contingency approach: the effects of spokesperson type and service type on service advertising perceptions. Journal of Advertising, 31 (2), 17–35.

Stephens, A., & Rice, A. (1998). Spicing up the message. Finance Week, 76 (26), 46–47.

Stuart, E. W., Shimp, T. A., & Engle, R. W. (1987). Classical conditioning of consumer attitudes: four experiments in an advertising context. Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (3), 334–349.

Szymanski, D. M., Troy, L. C., & Bharadwaj, S. G. (1995). Order of entry and business performance: an empirical synthesis and reexamination. Journal of Marketing, 59 (4), 17–33.

Tellis, G. J. (1988). The price elasticity of selective demand: a meta-analysis of econometric models of sales. Journal of Marketing Research, 25 (4), 331–341.

Thompson, S. G., & Sharp, S. J. (1999). Explaining heterogeneity in meta-analysis: a comparison of methods. Statistics in Medicine, 18 (20), 2693–2708.

*Till, B. D., Stanley, S. M., & Priluck, R. (2008). Classical conditioning and celebrity endorsers: an examination of belongingness and resistance to extinction. Psychology and Marketing, 25 (2), 179–196.

*Toncar, M., Reid, J. S., & Anderson, C. E. (2007). Effective spokespersons in a public service announcement. Journal of Communication Management, 11 (3), 258–275.

Vakratsas, D., & Ambler, T. (1999). How advertising works: what do we really know? Journal of Marketing, 63 (1), 26–43.

Van den Noortgate, W., López-López, J. A., Marín-Martínez, F., & Sánchez-Meca, J. (2014). Meta-analysis of multiple outcomes: a multilevel approach. Behavior Research Methods, 47 , 1274–1294.

Van Steenburg, E. (2015). Areas of research in political advertising: a review and research agenda. International Journal of Advertising, 34 (2), 195–231.

Veer, E., Becirovic, I., & Martin, B. A. (2010). If Kate voted Conservative, would you? European Journal of Marketing, 44 (3/4), 436–450.

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metaphor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36 (3), 1–48.

Viechtbauer, W. (2015). Package metafor. accessed September 8, 2015, [available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/metafor/metafor.pdf ].

*Wei, P. S., & Lu, H. P. (2013). An examination of the celebrity endorsements and online customer reviews influence female consumers’ shopping behavior. Computers in Human Behavior, 29 (1), 193–201.

*Wheeler, R. T. (2009). Nonprofit advertising: impact of celebrity connection, involvement and gender on source credibility and intention to volunteer time or donate money. Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing, 21 (1), 80–107.

Whittaker, J. O. (1965). Sex differences and susceptibility to interpersonal persuasion. The Journal of Social Psychology, 66 , 91–94.

*Wu, W.-Y., Linn, C. T., Fu, C. S., & Sukoco, B. M. (2012). The role of endorsers, framing, and rewards on the effectiveness of dietary supplement advertisements. Journal of Health Communication, 17 (1), 54–75.

*Yoon, D., & Kim, Y. K. (2016). Effects of self-congruity and source credibility on consumer responses to coffeehouse advertising. Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management, 25 (2), 167–196.

*Young, K., & Miller, Y. D. (2015). Keeping it natural: does persuasive magazine content have an effect on young women’s intentions for birth? Women & Health, 55 (4), 447–466.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Open access funding provided by University of Vienna.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Communication, University of Vienna, Währinger Straße 29, A-1090, Vienna, Austria

Johannes Knoll & Jörg Matthes

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jörg Matthes .

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Knoll, J., Matthes, J. The effectiveness of celebrity endorsements: a meta-analysis. J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. 45 , 55–75 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-016-0503-8

Download citation

Received : 18 February 2016

Accepted : 25 September 2016

Published : 12 October 2016

Issue Date : January 2017

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-016-0503-8

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Meta-analysis
  • Celebrity endorsements
  • Advertising effects
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Online star vs. celebrity endorsements: the role of self-concept and advertising appeal in influencing purchase intention.

\r\nPengfei Shi

  • 1 School of Economics and Management, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
  • 2 School of Economics and Management, Wuhan University, Wuhan, China
  • 3 Business School, Hubei University, Wuhan, China
  • 4 Department of Business Administration, Shandong Labor Vocational and Technical College, Jinan, China
  • 5 China Academy of Civil Aviation Science and Technology, Beijing, China

Despite the fact that companies increasingly value online star endorsements as Internet celebrity economy booms, scientific knowledge on the effect of online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention is limited. Based on the theories of self and construal level theory, this study investigates the impact of online star vs. celebrity endorsements on purchase intention and explores the underlying mechanism as well as boundary conditions. The results of four studies reveal the following: (1) Compared with no endorsement, both celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements lead to increases in consumers’ purchase intention, with no significant difference between the two. (2) Self-concept mediates these relationships; specifically, celebrity and online star endorsements activate the ideal and actual self respectively, and enhance consumers’ willingness to purchase. (3) The effect of endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention is moderated by advertising appeals. That is, celebrity endorsements enhance purchase intention when consumers are exposed to symbolic appeals in advertisements, and online star endorsements enhance purchase intention when it is matched with functional advertising appeals.

Introduction

Online stars, also known as Internet celebrities ( wang hong in Chinese) are becoming most popular with Internet users due to the rapid expansion of social media platforms, such as YouTube, Facebook, Sina Weibo, Tik Tok, and Kuaishou. While social celebrities remain popular influencers in mainstream markets and are highly valued by companies, online stars have increasingly begun to be deployed as endorsers, for example, Bella Hadid endorsing Nike, and Papi Jiang and Zhang Dayi advertising Weibo Story. Unlike traditional celebrities, online stars often thrive in ordinary social groups like common consumers, who perceive a sense of affinity and closeness with these stars ( Stever and Lawson, 2013 ; Clarke et al., 2016 ; Zhang et al., 2020 ).

Although many academic studies have confirmed the economic value of various endorser types ( Ayeh, 2015 ), little is known about the effectiveness of celebrity vs. online star endorsements. While researches have focused on the development of online stars, measurement of their influence, and relationships between online stars and fans ( Abidin and Thompson, 2012 ; Clarke et al., 2016 ), few have examined the effect of online stars and traditional celebrities on purchase intention. Further, as businesses increasingly rely on social media platforms to promote their products, consumers are changing their purchasing habits, their state of mind, cognition or perception ( Lăzăroiu et al., 2020 ). Thus, their self-concept and purchase intention are also influenced ( Verwijmeren et al., 2011 ; Krishna, 2012 ; Vrontis et al., 2021 ). Although the underlying processes of endorser effect have been explained by the source-attractiveness model, meaning transfer theory, matching theory, or elaboration likelihood model ( Erdogan, 1999 ; Erdogan and Baker, 2000 ; Kerr et al., 2015 ; Zamudio, 2016 ), little is known about the role of the activated self-concept—either the ideal self or actual self—on consumers’ purchase intention. What’s more, substantial literature has both categorized advertising appeals, such as rational and emotional appeals, hard-selling and soft-selling appeals, thought and sensory appeals, and symbolic and functional appeals ( Shavitt, 1992 ; Gauzente and Roy, 2012 ; Lantos, 2015 ), and discussed the moderating effects of these appeals on various factors like consumers, cultures, products, channels, and celebrities ( Chang, 2006 ; Choi et al., 2012 ; Vazifehdoust et al., 2014 ; Kunkel et al., 2018 ). However, research has remained largely dedicated to matching symbolic and functional appeals with different product categories but has failed to match them with celebrity and online star endorsements.

To address this research gap, the current study focuses on the effects of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention. Specifically, we address the following questions: (1) What is the effect of online star endorsement (vs. no endorsement) on purchase intention? Is there a significant difference between the effects of celebrity endorsement and online star endorsement on purchase intention? (2) What is the psychological mechanism underlying the effects of these two types of endorsements? (3) How are these effects moderated by symbolic and functional advertising appeals?

Theory and Hypotheses

Online stars and social celebrities.

Online stars, also called cyberspace stars, web celebrities, online celebrities, or web influencers, are usually focal figures who attract numerous fans by creating and enthusiastically sharing original texts, pictures, or videos on social media ( Stever and Lawson, 2013 ). They gain personal influence and public recognition by frequent interactions with fans, or present desires and wishes that are not fulfilled in practical life to draw massive attention ( Zhou et al., 2018 ). Despite their increasing influence in endorsements, online stars have not been consistently defined across scientific researches. The current study defines “online stars” as self-empowered individuals ( Tiggemann and Slater, 2013 ) who gain fame and recognition by featuring self-generated content and frequently interacting with a large follower base on social media ( Wood and Burkhalter, 2014 ; Schouten et al., 2020 ), becoming key opinion formers and online broadcasters and hence transforming social capital into cash.

Unlike online stars, social celebrities are often famous actors, singers, artists, supermodels, or athletes, etc., who gain social influence with their professional talent in specific fields and are frequently shown to general public via various media forms ( Driessens, 2013 ). With their positive images in traditional media channels and social interactions, they gain fame and reputation and are greatly favored by certain social groups ( Turner, 2004 ). Although online stars are sometimes also called “online/web celebrities,” the concept of “celebrities” is adopted in a more traditional sense in the current study, referring to individuals who become famous and popular by showing their personal talent and attractiveness, in conventional channels like movies, pictures, commercials and videos. As representatives of certain individual, social and commercial images, they are usually viewed by the public as cultural symbols.

Although celebrities and online stars share similarities, such as being considered to be popular, professional, and trustworthy ( Choi and Rifon, 2007 ; Khamis et al., 2017 ), the two groups actually differ greatly. Social celebrities normally gain public recognition via traditional and social media, while online stars build their reputation almost exclusively by social media platforms. Further, unlike celebrities who gain fame through contents in various media forms, online stars become self-empowered with their self-generated contents. What’s more, online stars are often “grassroots” people from all walks of life ( Russell and Rasolofoarison, 2017 ). Whereas traditional celebrities seem to be distant, online stars seem more like ordinary people and are perceived as closer to consumers. Hence, social distance , or the perceived psychological distance between the two types of endorsers and their fans, is the key factor differentiating celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements ( Stever and Lawson, 2013 ; Liu and Qin, 2014 ).

Celebrity Endorsements, Online Star Endorsements, and Consumers’ Purchase Intention

As the predictor of consumers’ buying behavior, purchase intention refers to the likelihood or desire of a customer to buy a certain product ( Burke et al., 1985 ). Numerous factors affect purchase intentions, such as consumer personality characteristics, product internal cues, product external cues, and consumption scenarios, etc. ( Dodds et al., 1991 ). Advertising, a combination of the above factors, has become an effective marketing tool. For both online and offline consumers, once they are exposed to different types of endorsement advertisements, they associate the content with the endorsed products, thereby increasing purchases intention.

Celebrity endorsement is a long-standing and effective driver of consumers’ purchase intention. Celebrities’ public image and fame engender trust in consumers and increase their willingness to purchase the endorsed products ( Ayeh, 2015 ). Moreover, celebrities embody special and symbolic meanings in certain sociocultural environments. Thus, symbolic meaning is injected into the products or brands that they endorse via the process of meaning transfer in consumer purchasing ( Knoll and Matthes, 2016 ). Consumers are also likely to buy products endorsed by celebrities due to the bandwagon and halo effects. In addition, research has demonstrated that advertisements with celebrity endorsements are associated with higher information processing speeds, better product evaluations and increased purchase intention, compared with advertisements lacking such endorsements ( Rs and Alawadhi, 2020 ). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H1: Compared with no endorsement, celebrity endorsements have a positive effect on consumers’ purchase intention.

Online star endorsements represent a new type of endorsement. Similar to conventional celebrities in the sense that both are considered credible, popular and professional, online stars are recruited by more and more companies to present information and recommend purchases to their huge fan bases through impression management, expressiveness and persuasiveness. Online star endorsements are statements made by online celebrities in support of products or brands via advertisements on both traditional and social media. Based on the extant literature on famous persons, we posit that online star endorsements promote consumers’ purchase intention and that the attractiveness or personal charisma of these stars directly influences purchase intention ( Park and Lin, 2020 ). More importantly, the unique qualities of online stars, such as their amiable, topical, and interactive nature, all contribute to consumers’ willingness to buy. Most online stars are considered “common” people and are naturally more amiable and appealing to consumers ( Levordashka and Utz, 2016 ; Jin and Ryu, 2020 ). Therefore, consumers are more likely to accept online stars’ product recommendations. In public discussions, online stars are topical, and their comments boost interactions, thereby promoting the dissemination of their recommendations among the fans ( Elmira and Oxana, 2018 ). Online stars also use social media frequently to communicate with fans, which further engenders trust from these fans, who unconsciously transfer this trust to the recommended product information ( Gong and Li, 2017 ). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H2: Compared with no endorsement, online star endorsements have a positive effect on consumers’ purchase intention.

Celebrity Endorsements, Online Star Endorsements, Ideal Self, and Actual Self

Self-concept is defined as a person’s cognitive and affective understanding of who and what they are ( Joseph, 1982 ). Individuals have multiple selves but see themselves as a holistic self that is activated and stabilized ( Wang et al., 2000 ). Following most literature on consumer research, we assume that consumers’ selves take the forms of an ideal self and an actual self ( Sirgy, 1982 ). The ideal self is shaped by imagined ideals and goals related to what the person believes that he would like to be or aspire to become, whereas the actual self is based on the person’s perceived self-reality ( Sirgy, 1982 ). When activated, self-concept determines one’s perception of and response to the environment. An individual is more willing to strengthen this self-concept and strive to seek self-enhancement or self-congruence through consuming behaviors, such as buying, presenting, or using certain products that are considered to represent an extension of the self. In this context, self-congruence theory postulates that consumers judge their self-image based on their perception of how closely their self-concept fits with the product’s image. Self-enhancement and self-congruence thus have been considered significant theories in analyses of the connection between consumer self-concept and purchase intention ( Ahn et al., 2013 ).

According to construal level theory , people make abstract mental construals of distal events that differ by the dimensions of psychological distance. Specifically, as psychological distance increases, people construe objects at a higher (more abstract) level. High-level construals, which include the general, superordinate and core features of events, are simpler, more abstract, schematic, and decontextualized representations. In contrast, low-level construals, which include the incidental and subordinate features of events, are relatively more complex, concrete, contextualized, and unstructured representations. Four dimensions of psychological distance are described in construal level theory: temporal distance, social distance, spatial distance, and hypotheticality ( Stephan et al., 2011 ). Despite important differences between these four dimensions, all of them affect the construal level similarly with respect to predicting different psychological states, as demonstrated in the literature ( Milkman et al., 2010 ). Studies have found that, temporal distance triggers different construal levels for people, leading to the production of different selves ( Kivetz and Tyler, 2007 ; Freitas et al., 2008 ; Rogers and Bazerman, 2008 ). The social distance generated by an individual’s perception of social objects should have a similar effect. Therefore, we introduce construal level theory to explain why celebrity endorsement and online star endorsement arouse different self-concepts.

When consumers view celebrity endorsements in advertisements, they focus on the celebrities, who are idols in their mind and are perceived to be socially distant. This triggers a high-level construal, motivating the consumer to represent themselves using abstract, core, essential and general features and generating a self-concept that congruent with their own or others’ expectations ( Stephan et al., 2011 ). In this way, celebrity endorsements inspire the ideal self, and consumers strive to maintain a stable and coherent ideal self as a means of self-reflection. As the buying process is an interaction between advertised product information and consumer self-image ( Knoll and Matthes, 2016 ), consumers are likely to change their attitude in response to the influence of celebrity endorsers. That is, activation of the ideal self leads consumers to pay attention to product self-presentation in advertisements. Congruence between product self-presentation and self-image drives consumers to associate the product with their personal image and social status, fostering a closer product–consumer connection ( Su and Reynolds, 2017 ). Consumers who perceive the product as a projection of their self-image and a symbol of the endorsing celebrity buy it to maintain, extend and enhance their ideal selves ( Hong and Zinkhan, 2010 ). Hence, the product is a better match for the ideal self and thus promotes purchase intention. Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H3: The effect of celebrity endorsements on purchase intention is mediated by consumers’ ideal self.

Similarly, when consumers view online star endorsements in advertisements, they focus on the online stars, who are perceived to be common persons or even friends. This perception of natural social closeness results in a low-level construal, diverting consumer s’ attention from inner features. Thus, consumers represent themselves using concrete, superficial, subordinate and contextualized psychological features and thus generating a self-concept at close range ( Qian and Park, 2021 ). In this way, online star endorsements activate the actual self, leading consumers to pay more attention to the actual functional value of the product, instead of its symbolic value. Strengthened as signifiers, online stars are more closely linked with the recommended products than with idols or dreams ( Khamis et al., 2017 ). To speed up the spread of information, these stars usually aim to facilitate the understanding of product information and thus highlight a product’s functional value. In this case, consumers are likely to buy products endorsed by online stars to achieve a congruent self-image and extend the actual self ( Sirgy, 1982 ). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H4: The effect of online star endorsements on purchase intention is mediated by the consumers’ actual self.

Moderating Effects of Symbolic Appeal and Functional Appeal

Although social media platforms, such as Sino Weibo, WeChat, and Kuaishou, provide new opportunities for celebrities to promote themselves and interact with fans, the mysteriousness, distance, and idolatry associated with these celebrities remain unaffected. They remain symbolic as distant idols and dreams that are not readily approachable. The social distance between consumers and celebrities leads to activation of the ideal self, which represents consumers’ expectations and ambition. As a result, consumers who purchase the product are willing to trade some functional value, or even precious resources like time and money, in exchange for rewards, such as increased expression of their personality, identity and image. In advertisements, symbolic appeal means more emphasis on representing symbolic meaning for consumers, on the basis of creating an emotional state of mind for consumers ( Johar and Sirgy, 1991 ; Choi et al., 2012 ). As symbolic appeal in advertising is anchored on creating ideal goals and images and delivering a certain lifestyle, social status, and signified meaning via products ( Wolter et al., 2016 ), it is a better match for consumers whose ideal self is activated by celebrity endorsements. Such consumers are more likely to purchase products endorsed by celebrities to achieve a more congruent and better extended ideal self ( Wang and Scheinbaum, 2018 ). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H5: Compared with functional appeal, symbolic appeal in advertisements strengthens the relationship between celebrity endorsements and consumers’ purchase intention.

Online stars use social media platforms to release pictures, texts and videos that represent real self-images and to interact frequently with their followers. As noted previously, online stars are perceived to be ordinary people, which reduces their social distance from consumers. These interactions are more likely to activate the actual self. To guide action, such consumers who purchase a product are more likely to sacrifice internal value for increased instrumental and practical value. Functional appeal in an advertisement involves informing consumers of the main functions, outstanding performance and comparative advantages of the product via concrete, peripheral, and functional messages ( Johar and Sirgy, 1991 ; Ashley and Tuten, 2015 ). In this case, consumers’ actual self is triggered by online star endorsements, resulting in a preference for functional appeal in advertisements. Such consumers are more likely to purchase products endorsed by online stars to achieve a more congruent and better extended actual self ( Ahn et al., 2013 ). Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis:

H6: Compared with symbolic appeal, functional appeal in advertisements strengthens the relationship between online star endorsements and consumers’ purchase intention.

Methods and Results

Study overview.

We conducted four studies to test our hypotheses. In Study 1, we examined the influences of celebrity and online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention and determined that both associations were positive. In Studies 2 and 3, we explored these positive associations and determined that they are mediated by consumers’ ideal self and actual self, respectively. Finally, in Study 4, we showed that this mediation effect is conditional upon the type of advertising appeals.

Measurements

To measure the constructs in our studies, we used or adapted established scales with reference to prior research (see Table 1 ). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) including all study constructs produced a satisfactory overall fit. Factor loadings (more than 0.700), Cronbach’s alphas (over 0.700), CRs (greater than 0.700), and AVEs (ranging from 0. 529 to 0. 783) all point to high levels of reliability and convergent validity of the measurements ( Table 1 ). In addition, the square root of the AVE value is higher than the correlation coefficients for all latent variables, indicating that high discriminant validity.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Construct measurements.

Pilot Study and Stimuli

We conducted a pilot study to determine the appropriate stimuli for formal experiments. To eliminate confounding variables and ensure good validity and reliability, we used real celebrities and online stars as the experimental dataset. We also drew on authoritative selection methods developed by scholars, such as Sengupta et al. (1997) and Biswas et al. (2006) to ensure that suitable stimuli were selected for this study.

First, we selected all celebrities who appeared in all rankings, such as Baidu Billboard, Tencent Billboard, and Weibo Billboard, ranked them in order, and finally identified 10 celebrities of different genders, excluding those who had received negative publicity. Second, using the same selection method, we listed 20 online stars who appeared in all rankings, such as China online stars big data report, listed them in general order and finally selected 10 online stars of different genders. Third, we invited 55 undergraduates (M age = 21.257 years; 30 female) at a comprehensive university in Wuhan to complete a questionnaire. In this questionnaire, we listed the names of the 10 stars and 10 online stars and attached their photos, which were modified using Photoshop to ensure that they were identical in size, brightness, pixels, and colors. To rule out effects of popularity, expertise, trustworthiness and attractiveness, we introduced the widely used celebrity trait scale developed by Ohanian (1991) and Ding et al. (2005) . Specifically, subjects rated on the popularity, expertise, trustworthiness and attractiveness of the celebrities and online stars on 7-point semantic differential scales. For example, expertise was measured by four items with 7-point scales: expert/not an expert, experienced/inexperienced, qualified/unqualified, skilled/unskilled.

Using data from 52 valid questionnaires (those that lacked key information or were answered sloppily were ruled out), we selected Zhao Liying and Papi Jiang, who received the highest scores, to represent female celebrities and online stars, respectively. As shown in Table 2 , we found no significant difference between the respondents’ perceptions of these stimuli in terms of popularity, expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. We further selected Yang Yang and Liu Yu, who also received the highest scores, to represent male celebrities and online stars, respectively. Similarly, we found no significant differences between the respondents’ perception of the common features of the celebrity and online star ( Table 2 ). To better allow the participants to distinguish between celebrities and online stars in the following formal experiments, we added a text description of each to the stimulus materials, drawing on the research methods of Atkin and Block (1983) .

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean scores of the properties of celebrities and online stars.

After setting our research stimuli, we processed photos of the celebrities, online stars and products as described earlier, and used Photoshop to combine these photos into single images. In each image, the celebrity or online star is shown on the left side and the product is shown in a relatively smaller shape on the right side.

In Study 1, we explored the effects of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention. We selected a facial cleanser endorsed both by celebrities and by online stars, using a fictitious brand X to rule out any effect of brand association.

Participants and Procedure

One hundred and twenty participants (recruited both on-campus and off-campus) were invited to complete a questionnaire in exchange for a bookmark of ¥2 as a gift. 117 valid questionnaires were collected (M age = 22.836 years, SD age = 4.472 years; 56.9% female). Three questionnaires were considered invalid because they lacked key information or were answered sloppily.

First, participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: celebrity endorsement, online star endorsement or no endorsement (shown in Figure 1 ). They were given materials describing certain conditions, as in the following example: “You want to buy a facial cleanser. When you are browsing information on a famous online shopping site, you see an advertisement for Brand X facial cleanser endorsed by celebrities [endorsed by online stars/that is not endorsed].” We described celebrities and online stars in the stimulus material and selected Zhao Liying and Papi Jiang as examples of celebrities and online stars, respectively. Specifically, in the celebrity endorsement materials we described as follows: “Zhao Liying is now a top star, known as the most popular actress.” In the online star endorsement materials, the description read “Papi Jiang is an online star with a large number of fans, who is rated as the most popular online personality.” Then, referring to the pilot study, we measured the common features of them, to rule out the effects of these characteristics. After that, the participants were asked to complete the remaining three parts of the questionnaire. In the first part, we presented advertisements featuring celebrities, online stars or no endorsers to the participants, who were then asked to rate items concerning obvious celebrity and online star characteristics, such as “This celebrity is viewed as an idol, with a strong sense of distance” ( Table 1 ). The items were adapted from scales by Hess (2003) ; Stever and Lawson (2013) , and Cohen et al. (2016) . In the second part, participants were asked to evaluate their purchase intention to purchase the product under the three conditions. This section was based on the scale by Dodds et al. (1991) and Ma et al. (2015) , with four items, such as “I will like the product recommended in this advertisement” ( Table 1 ). All items in this study were rated on 7-point Likert scales (1 = “strongly disagree,” 7 = “strongly agree”). In the last part, participants entered their demographic information.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Stimulus material in Study 1 (blurred for legal and copyright reasons). On a famous online shopping site, an advertisement for Brand X facial cleanser is endorsed by celebrities (is endorsed by online stars/is not endorsed). Image reproduced with permission from Rui Chu Cultural Development Co., Ltd.

Manipulation Check

We manipulated the perceptions of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements by assigning participants to different conditions. The results revealed that participants’ perceptions of the endorsing conditions were consistent with the types of endorsers. A significantly greater number of participants in the celebrity endorsement condition considered the advertisement as being endorsed by celebrities (M celebrity endorsement = 5.129, SD = 1.017; M online star endorsement = 3.397, SD = 0.747, p < 0.001). In the condition of online star endorsement, a significantly greater number of participants considered the advertisement as being endorsed by online stars (M celebrity endorsement = 2.939, SD = 0.887; M online star endorsement = 4.871, SD = 0.687, p < 0.001). Therefore, the manipulation of celebrities and online stars was effective. As shown in Table 2 , there were no significant differences between the participants’ perception of the common traits of celebrities and online stars (M celebrity popularity = 4.688, SD = 1.011; M online star popularity = 4.744, SD = 0.937, p = 0.807), (M celebrity expert ise = 4.564, SD = 0.795; M online star expert ise = 4.532, SD = 0.464, p = 0.843), (M celebrity trustworthiness = 4.462, SD = 1.115; M online star trustworthiness = 4.403, SD = 1.205, p = 0.830), (M celebrity attractiveness = 5.302, SD = 1.009; M online star attractiveness = 5.032, SD = 1.309, p = 0.340).

Hypothesis Testing

One-way ANOVA showed that differences in the product endorsers influenced participants’ willingness to purchase products [ F (1, 117) = 37.135, p < 0.001]. Compared with no endorsement, both celebrity endorsement (M celebrity endorsement = 4.903, SD = 0.784; M control = 3.613, SD = 0.656, p < 0.001) and online star endorsement increased participants’ purchase intention (M online star endorsement = 4.772, SD = 0.766; M control = 3.613, SD = 0.656, p < 0.001). These results support Hypotheses 1 and 2. The results also showed that the influences of celebrity endorsers and online star endorsers on consumers’ purchase intention were not significantly different.

The results of Study 1 mainly verified the effects of celebrity and online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention and proved a lack of significant difference between these effects. However, the factors driving this effect were not examined. Therefore, we conducted the following study to explore the mediating mechanism.

The main purpose of Study 2 was to verify that celebrity and online star endorsements activate consumers’ ideal self or actual self, respectively, thereby enhancing their purchase intention. Specific stimulus information for study 2 is available in Supplementary Appendix .

One hundred and fifty participants (recruited both on-campus and off-campus) were asked to complete the questionnaire in exchange for a bookmark of ¥2 as a gift. 137 valid questionnaires were collected (M age = 23.456 years, SD age = 4.451 years; 58.1% female). 13 questionnaires were ruled out because they lacked key information or were answered sloppily.

Like in Study 1, we selected Zhao Liying and Papi Jiang as endorsers in this study; however, we replaced the facial cleanser with a computer, which was fictitiously branded “X” to ensure the effectiveness of manipulation by avoiding associations with existing brands. We replicated the manipulation procedure involving all other pictures used in Study 1.

We also conducted an experiment to examine the psychological mechanism by which celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements influence purchasing behavior. We replicated all the materials (except the product) and steps used in the previous study and added a new step in which the participants were asked to score their ideal and actual selves using a 4-item scale developed by Cohen et al. (2016) . This step was taken after completion of the first two steps of the experiment. As shown in Table 1 , the constructs of ideal self and actual self were each measured by three items on 7-point Likert scales.

We first confirmed the effectiveness of all manipulations. The results revealed that a significantly greater number of participants in the celebrity endorsement condition considered the advertisement as being endorsed by traditional celebrities (M celebrity endorsement = 4.889, SD = 0.682; M online star endorsement = 2.924, SD = 0.782, p < 0.001). A significantly greater number of participants in the online star endorsement advertisement condition considered the advertisement as being endorsed by online stars (M online star endorsement = 5.095, SD = 1.002; M celebrity endorsement = 3.552, SD = 0.852, p < 0.001). Therefore, the manipulation of celebrities and online stars was effective. There were no significant differences in the participants’ perceptions of common traits between the celebrities and online stars (M celebrity popularity = 4.702, SD = 0.961; M online star popularity = 4.667, SD = 0.973, p = 0.705), (M celebrity expert ise = 4.533, SD = 1.103; M online star expert ise = 4.426, SD = 1.156, p = 0.671), (M celebrity trustworthiness = 4.521, SD = 0.501; M online star trustworthiness = 4.596, SD = 0.830, p = 0.640), (M celebrity attractiveness = 5.143, SD = 1.017; M online star attractiveness = 5.277, SD = 1.307, p = 0.617).

We conducted a one-way ANOVA to show that different product endorsers influenced the participants’ willingness to purchase the product [ F (1, 137) = 39.753, p < 0.001]. Compared with no endorsement, both celebrity endorsement (M celebrity endorsements = 4.893, SD = 0.748; M control = 3.694, SD = 0.664, p < 0.001) and online star endorsement led to increases in purchase intention (M online star endorsement = 4.760, SD = 0.767; M control = 3.694, SD = 0.664, p < 0.001). In different endorsing conditions, we found no significant difference in consumers’ purchase intention when the product was changed.

Mediation Analysis

Bootstrap analysis was adopted in the current research to test mediation, since causal step regression is limited to models with one mediation variable and continuous variables. As the main effect was verified in Study 1 and 2, referring to Schouten et al. (2020) ’s research process, we only reported the mediating effect results. With a sample size of 5,000 and 95% confidence interval (CI), the mediating effect of ideal self on celebrity endorsement was significant ( Figure 2 ), as the CI did not include 0 (BootLLCL = −0.239, BootULC = -0.090). In contrast, the mediating effect of actual self under this condition was not significant, as the CI contained 0 (BootLLC = −0.076, BootULCL = 0.019). Meanwhile, the mediating effect of actual self on online star endorsement was significant ( Figure 3 ), as the CI did not include 0 (BootLLCL = 0.026, BootULCL = 0.164), whereas the mediating effect of ideal self under this condition was not significant, with a CI containing 0 (BootLLC = −0.011, BootULC = 0.074). In summary, celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements led to activation of the participants’ ideal self and actual self, respectively, supporting Hypotheses 3 and 4.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediating analysis of celebrity endorsement on purchase intention.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mediating analysis of online star endorsement on purchase intention.

In addition to providing further support for Hypotheses 1 and 2, the results of Study 2 also provide support for Hypotheses 3 and 4, as they verified the mediating role of self-concept in the effects of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements on purchase intention. To further explore the mediating role of self-concept from a different perspective, we conducted Study 3 using a priming approach. In the following section, we describe the selection of male rather than female celebrities and online stars and further alteration of product types.

In this study, we followed the approach developed by Kettle and Häubl (2011) to activate the ideal and actual selves, by using the situation of drinking beer in a public or private location as the stimulus ( Graeff, 1997 ), which is an established way to activate self-concept.

We proved in Study 2 that celebrity endorsements activate consumers’ ideal self, whereas online star endorsements activate the actual self, both of which promote purchase intention. We then assume that, exposed to celebrity endorsements, consumers with activated ideal self are more willing to buy products endorsed by celebrities. Similarly, after exposure to online star endorsements, consumers with activated actual self are more likely to find online star-endorsing products attractive. To further test our assumptions, we conducted Study 3.

Two hundred participants (recruited both on-campus and off-campus) took part in the study in exchange for a bookmark of ¥2 as a gift. 183 valid questionnaires were collected (M age = 23.803 years, SD age = 5.173 years; 59.6% female). Seventeen questionnaires were ruled out because they lacked key information.

In Study 3, we replaced female endorsers with males, specifically Yang Yang and Liu Yu, as selected in the pilot study. Specific stimulus information for study 3 is available in Supplementary Appendix . Similar to study 1, study 3 included a description of male celebrities and online stars. The male celebrity is described as follows, “Yang Yang is a celebrity with a large number of fans and is the most popular celebrity.” As for online stars, participants read “Liu Yu, an online star with a large number of fans, is viewed as a popular online star.” We also replaced the computer used in Study 2 with shampoo, which was branded fictitiously as “X.” We used a 2 (ideal self vs. actual self) × 3 (celebrity vs. online star vs. control) between-subjects design to further test the mediation effect. First, based on the experimental method described by Graeff (1997) to activate ideal self and actual self, we assigned participants randomly to either the ideal self or the actual self group and exposed them to materials that encouraged activation of the ideal or the real self-concept, respectively. The material for ideal self condition read as follows, “Imagine you and your friends in a restaurant, bar, lounge, or other public places. You are considering to buy a dozen of beer and have decided to drink them in the public. That is to say, when you drink the beer, there will be a lot of people watching you and the way you drink beer.” For actual self condition, it read “Suppose you are considering to buy a dozen of beer, and then go home to drink them to relax. Namely, you will drink beer in a private place with no one else present (excluding family members). At this time, you can drink while watching a rented movie, your favorite TV shows, or sports events on TV.” Next, we asked the participants to complete the ideal and actual self scales and report their intention to buy the product. Similarly, we measured the popularity, expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness of both celebrities and online stars to rule out the effects of these factors.

We first confirmed the effectiveness of our manipulation. The results of our check revealed that the participants in the actual self group indeed perceived a more realistic version of themselves than those in the ideal self group (M actual self = 5.019, SD = 0.886; M ideal self = 3.500, SD = 0.863, p < 0.001). The participants in the ideal self group reported a higher ideal vs. actual self score (M ideal self = 5.008, SD = 0.672; M actual self = 3.119, SD = 0.922, p < 0.001). We further determined that the manipulation of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements is appropriate. Our results showed that participants in celebrity endorsement group indeed perceived the advertisement as being endorsed by a celebrity, scoring higher than online star endorsements (M celebrity endorsement = 4.961, SD = 0.671; M online star endorsement = 3.377, SD = 0.953, p < 0.001), whereas in online star endorsement group, participants reported significantly higher scores for online star endorsements than celebrity endorsements (M online star endorsement = 5.128, SD = 0.700; M celebrity endorsement = 3.190, SD = 0.765, p < 0.001). In addition, no significant difference was observed between the participants’ perceptions of the common properties of celebrities and online stars (shown in Table 2 ).

Mediating Effect Analysis

A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between consumer self-concept and endorsement type [ F (1, 183) = 12.184, p < 0.001]. Specifically, participants with activated ideal self-concept are more likely to be motivated to purchase the product with celebrity endorsements compared with those in the online star endorsement group and control group (M celebrity endorsement = 4.735, SD = 0.673; M online star endorsement = 4.092, SD = 0.835; M control = 3.518, SD = 0.909, p < 0.001) ( Figure 4 ). However, when actual self was activated, participants in the online star endorsement group had a higher purchase intention than those in the celebrity endorsement group and control group (M online star endorsement = 5.084, SD = 0.742; M celebrity endorsement = 4.483, SD = 0.571; M control = 3.654, SD = 0.738, p < 0.001) ( Figure 4 ). That is, online star endorsements increased purchase intention by activating consumers’ actual self-concept. The results supported Hypotheses 3 and 4.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effects of endorsement types and self-concept on purchase intention.

The results of Study 3 further proved that consumers’ self-concept is a key factor connecting celebrity and online star endorsements with consumers’ purchase intention. However, the first three experiments did not examine the boundary conditions of these effects. We conducted Study 4 to test the moderating effect of appeal types in advertisement.

In this study, to examine the conditional factors for the effects of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements on purchase intention, we introduced symbolic appeal and functional appeal in advertisements as moderating variables. We then sought appropriate types of verbal descriptions in advertisements. We conducted this study to test Hypotheses 5 and 6.

Three hundred adults (recruited both on-campus and off-campus) participated in exchange for a bookmark of ¥2 as a gift. 262 valid questionnaires were collected (M age = 23.145 years, SD age = 4.663 years; 39.3% female). Thirty-eight incomplete questionnaires were ruled out.

We selected celebrities and online stars using the same process described in Study 1, replacing facial cleanser used in that study with a mobile phone, which we branded fictitiously as “Y” to ensure the effectiveness of manipulation.

The experiment was most similar to that of Study 1, except that the moderating variables were included and measured. We employed a 2 (advertising appeal: symbolic vs. functional) × 3 (endorser: celebrity vs. online star vs. control) between-subjects design to further test the moderating effect. First, the participants were randomly assigned to four treatment groups and two control groups. They read materials similar to those used in Study 1, only with a different product type. Specific stimulus information for this study is available in Supplementary Appendix . Next, we manipulated the advertisement appeal by presenting stimulus materials to the participants and describing the symbolic or functional value accordingly. The symbolic appeal description was as follows: “This mobile phone makes you more attractive, helps expressing your personality fully in the crowd, creates a perfect image and ultimate experience for you.” For functional appeal, the description read: “This mobile phone is absolutely easy to use, with fast operating speed, long battery life, short charging time and ultra-wide camera angle, meeting various needs for work and entertainment.” We also measured popularity, expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness of both celebrities and online stars to rule out the effects of these characteristics. Then, we exposed the participants to advertisements endorsed by either celebrities or online stars, or with no endorsers and asked to them complete the measurement items. They also rated the materials in terms of symbolic appeal and functional appeal based on a scale developed by Bhat and Reddy (1998) . As shown in Table 1 , symbolic appeal and functional appeal were each measured with three items on 7-point Likert scales (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree), such as “The products recommended in this advertisement are for people who want the best things in life.”

A one-way ANOVA was used to confirmed the effectiveness of our manipulation. As expected, participants in the celebrity endorsement group reported significantly higher scores for celebrity endorsements than for online star endorsements (M celebrity endorsement = 5.221, SD = 0.845; M online star endorsement = 3.546, SD = 0.769, p < 0.001). Participants in the online star endorsement group reported significantly higher scores for online star endorsements than for celebrity endorsement (M online star endorsement = 5.039, SD = 0.715; M celebrity endorsement = 3.071, SD = 0.852, p < 0.001).

In symbolic appeal group, participants assigned significantly higher scores to symbolic appeal than to functional appeal (M symbolic appeal = 5.118, SD = 0.666; M functional appeal = 3.541, SD = 0.982, p < 0.001). Conversely, in functional appeal group, participants assigned significantly higher scores to functional appeal than to symbolic appeal (M functional appeal = 5.181, SD = 0.707; M symbolic appeal = 3.216, SD = 0.724, p < 0.001). Further, as shown in Table 2 , participants did not perceive significant differences between the common traits of celebrities and online stars.

Moderating Effect Analysis

The results of the interaction test showed a significant moderating effect [ F (1, 262) = 19.328, p < 0.001]. Specifically, in the symbolic appeal condition, celebrity endorsements had a significantly stronger effect on purchase intention (M celebrity endorsement = 5.625, SD = 0.450) than online star endorsements (M online star endorsements = 4.514, SD = 0.558) and no endorsement (M control = 3.457, SD = 0.834) ( p < 0.001) ( Figure 5 ). In the functional appeal condition, online star endorsement had a significantly stronger effect on purchase intention (M online star endorsement = 5.668, SD = 0.453) than celebrity endorsement (M celebrity endorsement = 4.440, SD = 0.498) and no endorsement (M control = 3.617, SD = 0.705) ( p < 0.001) ( Figure 5 ). In other words, symbolic appeal is more closely matched with celebrity endorsements as a driver of consumers’ purchase intention, while functional appeal is more closely matched with online star endorsements, thus supporting Hypotheses 5 and 6.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effects of endorsement types and advertising appeals on purchase intention.

Following the first three studies, we introduced advertising appeal as a moderating variable in Study 4. Our results revealed that the mechanism underlying the influences of different types of endorsement on consumers’ purchase intention differs for advertisements involving symbolic appeal vs. functional appeal.

General Discussion

Research on celebrity endorsement generally suggest that popularity, expertise and trustworthiness increase consumers’ purchase intention ( Carroll and Ahuvia, 2006 ; Wang and Scheinbaum, 2018 ). However, few studies have focused on the effects of celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements on purchase intention ( Jin and Ryu, 2020 ; Schouten et al., 2020 ). We hypothesized that self-concept and advertisement appeal play significant roles in shaping consumers’ purchase intention. Across four studies, we confirmed the following: (1) Both celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements increase consumer purchase intention, in ways that are not significantly different. Similar to traditional celebrities, online stars also attract and convince consumers using their popularity, trustworthiness and expertise ( Zamudio, 2016 ). Furthermore, online stars interact more frequently with consumers and are therefore considered more like friends, enhancing consumers’ reliance on and trust in online star endorsements and drives purchase intention. (2) Self-concept plays a mediating role in this process. Consumers whose ideal self-concept is activated by celebrities are more willing to buy products endorsed by celebrities, who are perceived to be socially distant. However, consumers whose actual self-concept is activated by online stars are more likely to purchase products endorsed by them. (3) Matching between different advertising appeals and endorsement types relates to the effects of endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention. When ideal self is activated, consumers seek a product reward related to personality or identity-expression. As a symbolic appeal in an advertisement conveys social status and meaning of the product, consumers’ willingness to buy increases when this appeal is matched with a celebrity endorsement. Similarly, consumers’ purchase intention is higher when a functional appeal is matched with online star endorsements because consumers seek instrumental and utilitarian product rewards once their actual self has been activated.

Theoretical Contributions

The current research is among the first to precisely define the concept of online stars and differentiate it from the concept of celebrities in a traditional sense. As most prior studies focus on the effects of celebrity endorsements ( Atkin and Block, 1983 ; Biswas et al., 2006 ), the influence of online star endorsements is rather understudied due to limited differentiation between the two groups. Though online stars are distinct from celebrities in the sense that they are socially much closer to consumers, our research suggests that celebrity and online star endorsements are not significantly different in their ability to increase consumers’ purchase intention. Seemingly incongruent with Schouten et al. (2020) ’s findings, where significant difference in trustworthiness was found between online stars and celebrities, our study shows that both groups influence purchase intention likewise, as we understand online stars as similarly trustworthy to traditional celebrities since they are both famous to the public.

A second contribution is the importance of exploring the mechanism underlying the effects of online star endorsements and celebrity endorsements. Based on the theories of self and construal level theory, our results show that self-concept mediates the influence of online star endorsements and celebrity endorsements on purchase intention. While extant literature on endorsement effects seem rather confined to investigating the expertise, popularity, trustworthiness, and attractiveness of the endorsers ( Park and Lin, 2020 ; Schouten et al., 2020 ), our research provides a new perspective by examining consumers’ self-concept in different endorsing situations, supplementing the literature on self-concept and consumer purchase intention ( Oumlil and Erdem, 1997 ).

Third, instead of considering emotional/rational appeal as boundary conditions like previous endorsement studies did ( Ruiz and Sicilia, 2004 ; Andreu et al., 2015 ), we have found a better moderator using symbolic/functional appeal when investigating celebrity/online star endorsements effects. By examining the moderating effect of advertising appeals, we have gained a better understanding of the situations wherein celebrity endorsements and online star endorsements increase purchase intention.

Practical Implications

Our research has important practical implications for advertising and product marketing researches. When considering endorsers, managers may select both celebrities and online stars in accordance with current marketing practice ( Milkman et al., 2010 ), as they influence purchase intention in a similarly significant way. For example, Mercedes-Benz selected traditional celebrities and a popular streamer to endorse its products.

Second, our results also suggest managers consider the significance of appropriately matching the advertising appeal with the endorser type. Specifically, companies may adopt symbolic appeal when a celebrity endorses their product, reinforcing consumers’ ideal self and promoting purchase intention. Similarly, using functional appeal to match online star endorsers, companies activate consumers’ actual self and drive them to buy.

Third, enterprises should consider promoting purchase intention by activating consumer self-concept, which is dynamic and malleable ( Freitas et al., 2008 ; Hong and Zinkhan, 2010 ). Different activation of consumers’ self-concept increases their willingness to buy products endorsed by either celebrities or online stars to maintain a congruent self. Therefore, celebrity-endorsed products might describe themselves for benefits, such as personal charisma or perfect image in public places, to activate the ideal self. Likewise, companies using online star endorsements to activate the actual self, might encourage consumers to purchase by providing information on product core functions to meet their diverse needs.

Limitations and Future Research

This research also has some limitations. First, research on online stars remains at a nascent stage. Although we included celebrities and online stars of both sexes in our studies, we failed to include online stars of an older age because most of them are between 18 and 35. Future research might examine the influence of celebrity endorsers and online star endorsers of different ages on consumers’ purchase intention as the pool of online stars expands. Second, this research only validates the interaction of advertising appeals with endorsement types. Future research could examine other possible moderating variables, such as consumer cultural factors, consumer involvement, communication channels and consumption contexts. Third, this research only measures the independent effects of celebrity or online star endorsers. Future studies could investigate the influence of using a combination of celebrity and online star endorsements on consumers’ purchase intention.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the School of Economics and Management, Xinan University, China. The participants gave written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

PS was involved in the whole process of the research. XL, CS, LW, and BG contributed to conduct the experiments and perform the statistical analysis. PS, XL, and YZ wrote, revised, read the manuscript, and reviewed and coordinated the final article as a whole. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

This research was funded by the Humanity and Social Science Youth Foundation of Ministry of Education (No. 20XJC790006) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (No. SWU1909798).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the members of our team who has spent lots of time on this study, and we also thank Y. Z.’s team for the careful modification of the grammar of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736883/full#supplementary-material

Abidin, C., and Thompson, E. C. (2012). Buymylife.com: Cyber-femininities and commercial intimacy in blogshops. Wom. Stud. Int. Forum 35, 467–477. doi: 10.1016/j.wsif.2012.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahn, T., Ekinci, Y., and Li, G. (2013). Self-congruence, functional congruence, and destination choice. J. Busin. Res. 66, 719–723. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.09.009

Andreu, L., Casado-Díaz, A. B., and Mattila, A. S. (2015). Effects of message appeal and service type in csr communication strategies. J. Busin. Res. 68, 1488–1495. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.01.039

Ashley, C., and Tuten, T. (2015). Creative strategies in social media marketing: An exploratory study of branded social content and consumer engagement. Psychol. Market . 32, 15–27. doi: 10.1002/mar.20761

Atkin, C., and Block, M. (1983). Effectiveness of celebrity endorsers. J. Advertis. Res. 23, 57–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1983.tb00704.x

Ayeh, J. K. (2015). Travellers’ acceptance of consumer-generated media: An integrated model of technology acceptance and source credibility theories. Comput. Hum. Behav. 48, 173–180. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.049

Bhat, S., and Reddy, S. K. (1998). Symbolic and functional positioning of brands. J. Consu. Market. 15, 32–43. doi: 10.1108/07363769810202664

Biswas, D., Biswas, A., and Das, N. (2006). The differential effects of celebrity and expert endorsements on consumer risk perceptions: The role of consumer knowledge, perceived congruency, and product technology orientation. J. Advertis. 35, 17–31. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2006.10639231

Burke, J. J., Hatfield, J. L., Klein, R. R., and Mullet, J. E. (1985). Accumulation of heat shock proteins in field-grown cotton. Plant Physiol . 78, 394–398. doi: 10.1104/pp.78.2.394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, B. A., and Ahuvia, A. C. (2006). Some antecedents and outcomes of brand love. Market. Lett. 17, 79–89. doi: 10.1007/s11002-006-4219-2

Chang, C. (2006). Cultural masculinity/femininity influences on advertising appeals. J. Advertis. Res. 46, 315–323. doi: 10.2501/S0021849906060296

Choi, H., Yoon, H. J., Paek, H. J., and Reid, L. N. (2012). ‘Thinking and feeling’ products and ‘utilitarian and value-expressive’ appeals in contemporary TV advertising: a content analytic test of functional matching and the fcb model. J. Market. Commun. 18, 91–111. doi: 10.1080/13527266.2010.484581

Choi, S. M., and Rifon, N. J. (2007). Who is the celebrity in advertising? Understanding dimensions of celebrity images. J. Popu. Cult. 40, 304–324. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5931.2007.00380.x

Clarke, T. B., Murphy, J., and Adler, J. (2016). Celebrity chef adoption and implementation of social media, particularly pinterest: A diffusion of innovations approach. Int. J. Hospital. Manage. 57, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2016.06.004

Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., and Steele, C. M. (2016). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1151–1164.

Google Scholar

Ding, X. Q., Wang, H. M., and Ma, M. C. (2005). The effect of morality reputatidn on evaluations to celebrity endorser and endorsfment. Acta. Psychol. Sin. 37, 382–389.

Dodds, W. B., Monroe, K. B., and Grewal, D. (1991). Effects of price, brand, and store information on buyers, product evaluations. J. Market. Res . 28, 307–319. doi: 10.1177/002224379102800305

Driessens, O. (2013). Celebrity capital: Redefining celebrity using field theory. Theo. Soc. 42, 543–560.

Elmira, D., and Oxana, T. (2018). ‘Instafamous’–credibility and self-presentation of micro-celebrities on social media. Inform. Commun. Soc. 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/1369118X.2018.1438491

Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: A literature review. J. Market. Manage. 15, 291–314. doi: 10.1362/026725799784870379

Erdogan, B. Z., and Baker, M. J. (2000). Towards a practitioner-based model of selecting celebrity endorsers. Int. J. Advertis. 19, 25–42. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2000.11104782

Freitas, A. L., Langsam, K. L., Clark, S., and Moeller, S. J. (2008). Seeing oneself in one’s choices: construal level and self-pertinence of electoral and consumer decisions. J. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1174–1179. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.02.011

Gauzente, C., and Roy, Y. (2012). Message content in keyword campaigns, click behavior, and price-consciousness: A study of millennial consumers. J. Retail. Consu. Serv . 19, 78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2011.09.003

Gong, W., and Li, X. (2017). Engaging fans on microblog: The synthetic influence of parasocial interaction and source characteristics on celebrity endorsement. Psychol. Market. 34, 720–732. doi: 10.1002/mar.21018

Graeff, T. R. (1997). Consumption situations and the effects of brand image on consumers’ brand evaluations. Psychol.Market. 14, 49–70. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199701)14:13.0.CO;2-O

Hess, J. A. (2003). Measuring distance in personal relationships: The relational distance index. Perso. Relations 10, 197–216. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00046

Hong, J. W., and Zinkhan, G. M. (2010). Self-concept and advertising effectiveness: The influence of congruency, conspicuousness, and response mode. Psychol. Market. 12, 53–77. doi: 10.1002/mar.4220120105

Jin, S. V., and Ryu, E. (2020). “I’ll buy what she’s wearing”: The roles of envy toward and parasocial interaction with influencers in Instagram celebrity-based brand endorsement and social commerce. J. Retail. Consu. Serv. 55, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102121

Johar, J. S., and Sirgy, M. J. (1991). Value-expressive versus utilitarian advertising appeals: When and why to use which appeal. J. Advertis. 20, 23–33. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1991.10673345

Joseph, S. M. (1982). Self-concept in consumer behavior: a critical review. J. Consu. Res. 9, 287–300. doi: 10.1086/208924

Kerr, G., Schultz, D. E., Kitchen, P. J., Mulhern, F. J., and Beede, P. (2015). Does traditional advertising theory apply to the digital world? A replication analysis questions the relevance of the elaboration likelihood model. J. Advertis. Res. 55, 390–400. doi: 10.2501/JAR-2015-001

Kettle, K. L., and Häubl, G. (2011). The signature effect: Signing influences consumption-related behavior by priming self-identity. J. Consu. Res. 38, 474–474. doi: 10.1086/659753

Khamis, S., Ang, L., and Welling, R. (2017). Self-branding, “micro-celebrity” and the rise of Social Media Influencers. Celebr. Stud. 8, 191–208. doi: 10.1080/19392397.2016.1218292

Kivetz, Y., and Tyler, T. R. (2007). Tomorrow I’ll be me: the effect of time perspective on the activation of idealistic versus pragmatic selves. Organiz. Behav. HuM. Decis. Proces. 102, 193–211. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.07.002

Knoll, J., and Matthes, J. (2016). The effectiveness of celebrity endorsements: A meta-analysis. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 60, 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s11747-016-0503-8

Krishna, A. (2012). An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. J. Consu. Psychol. 22, 332–351. doi: 10.1051/shsconf/20173300022

Kunkel, T., Walker, M., and Hodge, C. M. (2018). The influence of advertising appeals on consumer perceptions of athlete endorser brand image. Eur. Spo. Manage. Q. 30, 1–23. doi: 10.1080/16184742.2018.1530688

Lantos, G. P. (2015). Consumer behavior in action: Real-life applications for marketing managers. Milton Park: Routledge Press.

Lăzăroiu, G., Neguriţă, O., Grecu, I., Grecu, G., and Mitran, P. C. (2020). Consumers’ decision-making process on social commerce platforms: Online trust, perceived risk, and purchase intentions. Front. Psychol. 11:890. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00890

Levordashka, A., and Utz, S. (2016). Ambient awareness: From random noise to digital closeness in online social networks. Comput. Hum. Behav. 60, 147–154. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.037

Liu, T., and Qin, H. (2014). Detecting and tagging users’ social circles in social media. Multime. Syst. 22, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s00530-014-0435-4

Ma, Z., Gill, T., and Jiang, Y. (2015). Core versus peripheral innovations: The effect of innovation locus on consumer adoption of new products. J. Market. Res. 52, 309–324. doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0337

Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., and Bazerman, M. H. (2010). I’ll have the ice cream soon and the vegetables later: A study of online grocery purchases and order lead time. Market. Lett. 21, 17–35. doi: 10.1080/10696679.1997.11501746

Ohanian, R. (1991). The impact of celebrity spokesperson’s perceived image on consumers’ intention to purchase. J. Advertis. Res. 31, 46–52.

Oumlil, A. B., and Erdem, O. (1997). Self-concept by gender: A focus on male-female consumer. J. Market. Theo. Pract. 5, 7–14. doi: 10.1080/10696679.1997.11501746

Park, H. J., and Lin, L. M. (2020). The effects of match-ups on the consumer attitudes toward internet celebrities and their live streaming contents in the context of product endorsement. J. Retail. Consu. Servi . 52, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2019.101934

Qian, J., and Park, J. S. (2021). Influencer-brand fit and brand dilution in china’s luxury market: The moderating role of self-concept clarity. J. Busin. Manage. 1, 1–22.

Rogers, T., and Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Future lock-in: Future implementation increases selection of “should” choices. Organiz. Behav. Hum. Decis. Proces. 106, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.08.001

Rs, M., and Alawadhi, R. (2020). Effect of celebirity endorsement on consumers purchase intention in the mediation effect of brand image. J. Busin. Res. 12, 454–468. doi: 10.20491/isarder.2020.855

Ruiz, S., and Sicilia, M. (2004). The impact of cognitive and/or affective processing styles on consumer response to advertising appeals. J. Busin. Res. 57, 657–664. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(02)00309-0

Russell, C. A., and Rasolofoarison, D. (2017). Uncovering the power of natural endorsements: A comparison with celebrity-endorsed advertising and product placements. Int. J. Advertis. 36, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2017.1348033

Schouten, A. P., Janssen, L., and Verspaget, M. (2020). Celebrity vs. Influencer endorsements in advertising: the role of identification, credibility, and Product-Endorser fit. Int. J. Advertis . 39, 258–281. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2019.1634898

Sengupta, J., Goodstein, R. C., and Boninger, D. S. (1997). All cues are not created equal: Obtaining attitude persistence under low-involvement conditions. J. Consu.Res. 23, 351–361. doi: 10.1086/209488

Shavitt, S. (1992). Evidence for predicting the effectiveness of value-expressive versus utilitarian appeals: A reply to Johar and Sirgy. J. Advertis . 21, 47–51. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1992.10673367

Sirgy, M. J. (1982). Self-concept in consumer behavior: A critical review. J. Consu. Res. 9, 287–300.

Stephan, E., Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (2011). The effects of time perspective and level of construal on social distance. J. Exper. Soc. Psychol. 47, 397–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.11.001

Stever, G. S., and Lawson, K. (2013). Twitter as a way for celebrities to communicate with fans: Implications for the study of parasocial interaction. Nor. Am. J. Psychol . 15, 339–354.

Su, N., and Reynolds, D. (2017). Effects of brand personality dimensions on consumers, perceived self-image congruity and functional congruity with hotel brands. Int. J. Hospital. Manage . 66, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2017.06.006

Tiggemann, M., and Slater, A. (2013). Netgirls: The internet, facebook, and body image concern in adolescent girls. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 630–633. doi: 10.1002/eat.22141

Turner, G. (2004). Understanding Celebrity. London: Sage Publications.

Vazifehdoust, H., Najafi, K., Najafi, M., and Behboudi, M. (2014). Using rational and emotional appeals in online advertisements for muslim customers. J. Isla. Market. 5, 97–124. doi: 10.1108/JIMA-07-2012-0039

Verwijmeren, T., Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., and Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2011). The workings and limits of subliminal advertising: The role of habits. J. Consu. Psychol . 21, 206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.11.004

Vrontis, R., Makrides, A., Christofi, M., and Thrassou, A. (2021). Social media influencer marketing: A systematic review, integrative framework and future research agenda. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 45, 617–644. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12647

Wang, C., Bristol, T., Mowen, J., and Chakraborty, G. (2000). Alternative modes of self-construal: Dimensions of connectedness-separateness and advertising appeals to the cultural and gender-specific self. J. Consu. Psychol. 9, 107–115. doi: 10.1207/S15327663JCP0902_5

Wang, S. W., and Scheinbaum, A. C. (2018). Enhancing brand credibility via celebrity endorsement: Trustworthiness trumps attractiveness and expertise. J. Advertis. Res. 58, 16–32. doi: 10.2501/JAR-2017-042

Wolter, J. S., Brach, S., Cronin, J. J. J., and Bonn, M. (2016). Symbolic drivers of consumer–brand identification and disidentification. J. Busin. Resea. 69, 785–793. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.07.011

Wood, N. T., and Burkhalter, J. N. (2014). Tweet this, not that: A comparison between brand promotions in microblogging environments using celebrity and company-generated tweets. J. Market. Communicat. 20, 129–146. doi: 10.1080/13527266.2013.797784

Zamudio, C. (2016). Matching with the stars: How brand personality determines celebrity endorsement contract formation. Int. J. Res. Market. 33, 409–427. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2015.07.002

Zhang, L., Kim, S. J., and Lee, J. H. (2020). A Study on the short videos that continue to affect the popularity factors of the Chinese Internet Celebrity-Papi Jiang. J. Asia. Stu. 23, 65–88. doi: 10.21740/jas.2020.02.23.1.65

Zhou, Y. F., Zhang, T., and Chen, S. N. (2018). Social communication and sentiment analysis of Web Celebrities: A case study of web popular milk tea brand HEYTEA. J. Busin. Econom . 38, 70–80. doi: 10.14134/j.cnki.cn33-1336/f.2018.04.007

Keywords : celebrity endorsement, online star endorsement, self-concept, purchase intention, advertising appeal

Citation: Shi P, Lu X, Zhou Y, Sun C, Wang L and Geng B (2021) Online Star vs. Celebrity Endorsements: The Role of Self-Concept and Advertising Appeal in Influencing Purchase Intention. Front. Psychol. 12:736883. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736883

Received: 23 July 2021; Accepted: 18 October 2021; Published: 23 November 2021.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2021 Shi, Lu, Zhou, Sun, Wang and Geng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaojing Lu, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

COMMENTS

  1. An Examination of Celebrity Product Involvement and Endorsement

    Nichols' (2021) study was that the more of a celebrity's personality that is passed to the product, the more influence the endorsement had on the consumer. Subsequently, the. researcher found there were four unique sources of involvement with the most impactful. being an owner of a company, followed by a collaboration.

  2. PDF Celebrity Endorsements and Advertising Effectiveness: the ...

    celebrity endorsement studies. Source variables, i.e., credibility (which includes expertise and trustworthiness) and attractiveness (which includes familiarity, similarity, and likeability), on the other hand, have been used by many researchers to understand the impact of celebrity endorsements on these key dependent variables. The underlying

  3. Social Media, Celebrity Endorsers and Effect on Purchasing Intentions

    (Silvera & Austad, 2004). Endorsement payments for celebrities have been as high as $45 million in 1998 for Michael Jordan, $25 million for Tiger Woods, $17 million for Grant Hill, and $10 million for Shaquille O'Neal (Daneshvary & Schwer, 2000). Age demographics play a major role in how a consumer is influenced by celebrity-based endorsements.

  4. The effect of athlete celebrity endorsement on attitude toward brand

    Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 2018. The effect of athlete celebrity endorsement on attitude toward brand and purchase intention. Yunheui Jeon. [email protected]. Follow this and additional works at: https://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Marketing Commons, Sports Management Commons, and the Sports Studies Commons.

  5. PDF Influence of celebrity endorsement/endorsers on consumer behaviour: A

    micro-celebrity beauty product endorsements, and their influence on consumers' purchasing behaviours. The Source Credibility model was used in this study to identify themes when analysing user comments. The study included five celebrity and five micro-celebrity influencers as case studies. Instagram is used as the data collection platform from

  6. PDF Thesis- Celebrity endorsement and purchase intent

    a positive, yet weak correlation between pre-scandal attitudes towards the celebrity and post-scandal. purchase intent since the correlation is positive r = 0.208 and the p value is less than chosen significance. value of 0.05, as the confidence level is 95%. The Pearson correlation of r = 0.437 and the significance.

  7. The Influence of Celebrity Endorsement and Purchase Intention Submitted

    the influence of celebrity endorsement on consumers' view of fashion products and purchase intention a thesis submitted to the graduate school in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree masters of science by chih-yu chen dr. harden - advisor ball state university

  8. The effectiveness of celebrity endorsements: a meta-analysis

    Celebrities frequently endorse products, brands, political candidates, or health campaigns. We investigated the effectiveness of such endorsements by meta-analyzing 46 studies published until April 2016 involving 10,357 participants. Applying multilevel meta-analysis, we analyzed celebrity endorsements in the context of for-profit and non-profit marketing. Findings revealed strong positive and ...

  9. PDF Study on Destination Attitudes

    1.3 Thesis structure 3 2 Literature review 4 2.1 Theoretical framework 4 2.2 Celebrity Endorsement 5 2.2.1 Definition & Background 5 2.2.2 Characteristics 7 2.2.3 Advantages 10 2.2.4 Disadvantages 11 2.2.5 Future Prospects 13 2.3 Destination Marketing 14 2.3.1 Destination Marketing Organizations 14 2.3.2 Social Media Marketing 17 2.3.3 ...

  10. (PDF) Celebrity Endorsement Influence on Brand ...

    Celebrity endorsement as defined by. Okafor (2011) is a technique of persuasion where customers relate to pe rson used or featured. in an advert. Celebrity endorsement is most e ffectively ...

  11. PDF The Effect of Celebrity Endorsement Effectiveness On

    on to purchase the product that being advertisement. The variable is the celebrity endorsement variables namely attractiveness, trustworthiness a. d expertise of the model in the print advertisement. These variables is later was after tested on its association with the independent. ariable which is the attitude towards advertisement. A que.

  12. PDF EXPLORING THE IMPACT OF CELEBRITY EN- DORSEMENT AND PRODUCT ...

    to the most famous luxury watch brand Rolex (Rolex 2013) celebrity endorsement seems to know no boundaries. In this study, a celebrity is simply defined as an individu-al who enjoys wide public recognition (McCracken 1989, 310). Correspondingly, celeb-rity endorsement is defined simply as famous person making a paid appearance with a

  13. PDF The Effect of Celebrity Endorsement Effectiveness On

    Trustworthiness placed on the celebrity endorser is referring to the honesty, integrity and. the perspective. of consumers (Erdogan et. al, 2001;Erdogan, 1999). Consumers strongly. feel that celebrity needs to be trustworthy in orderto them to accept the endorsement message f.

  14. The Impact of Celebrity Endorsement on Consumer Purchase Intention: An

    This study investigates the impact of celebrity endorsement and the moderating effect of negative publicity on consumer purchase intention or attitudes in an emerging market. Employing a quantitative research design, 500 respondents were surveyed using a structured questionnaire. The findings indicate that a celebrity endorser who has ...

  15. (PDF) Celebrity endorsement theories: Review perspective

    2. Literature Review. Abstract: The goal of this study is to examine existing theories in the field of celebrity endorsement and show how these. theories can be u sed as a foundation for futu re ...

  16. PDF Celebrity Endorsement: case study of J. Lindeberg

    This thesis is divided into six chapters starting with the introduction in the first chapter. The second chapter provides the reader with a literature review on previous research within the area of celebrity endorsement and closes with the conceptualization and the emergence of a frame of reference.

  17. (PDF) Celebrity Endorsement and Purchase Intentions: The Role of

    This paper aims. to examine the e ect of celebrity endorsement (i.e. attractiveness, credibility and product. match-up), perceived quality and brand loyalty on consumer purchase int entions. This ...

  18. Consistency and Inconsistency in Celebrity Endorsements

    Veras Victoria, Vanessa, "Consistency and Inconsistency in Celebrity Endorsements" (2015). Thesis. Rochester Institute of Technology. Accessed from This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the RIT Libraries. For more information, please contact [email protected]. RIT Digital Institutional Repository

  19. Celebrity endorsements: a literature review and research agenda

    The review identifies six areas of research on celebrity endorsements (celebrity prevalence, campaign management, financial effects, celebrity persuasion, non-evaluative meaning transfer, and brand-to-celebrity transfer). A review of the research in each area identifies key findings, conflicting results, and research gaps. ...

  20. Celebrity Endorsement

    In this chapter celebrity endorsement will be further explored with the use of several determinants. This section describes which factors a celebrity should have to be a solid endorser. To explain the determinants of celebrity endorsement, at the this thesis will look determinants illustrated in Amos et al. (2008).

  21. Frontiers

    A one-way ANOVA was used to confirmed the effectiveness of our manipulation. As expected, participants in the celebrity endorsement group reported significantly higher scores for celebrity endorsements than for online star endorsements (M celebrity endorsement = 5.221, SD = 0.845; M online star endorsement = 3.546, SD = 0.769, p < 0.001).

  22. (PDF) Relationship between Celebrity Endorsement and Impulse Buying

    unpublished thesis, Seoul National University, Seoul. Consult Park link. Cohen ... successful celebrity endorsement is a combination of attributes such as trustworthiness, expertise, similarity ...