Child Care and Early Education Research Connections

Pre-experimental designs.

Pre-experiments are the simplest form of research design. In a pre-experiment either a single group or multiple groups are observed subsequent to some agent or treatment presumed to cause change.

Types of Pre-Experimental Design

One-shot case study design, one-group pretest-posttest design, static-group comparison.

A single group is studied at a single point in time after some treatment that is presumed to have caused change. The carefully studied single instance is compared to general expectations of what the case would have looked like had the treatment not occurred and to other events casually observed. No control or comparison group is employed.

A single case is observed at two time points, one before the treatment and one after the treatment. Changes in the outcome of interest are presumed to be the result of the intervention or treatment. No control or comparison group is employed.

A group that has experienced some treatment is compared with one that has not. Observed differences between the two groups are assumed to be a result of the treatment.

Validity of Results

An important drawback of pre-experimental designs is that they are subject to numerous threats to their  validity . Consequently, it is often difficult or impossible to dismiss rival hypotheses or explanations. Therefore, researchers must exercise extreme caution in interpreting and generalizing the results from pre-experimental studies.

One reason that it is often difficult to assess the validity of studies that employ a pre-experimental design is that they often do not include any control or comparison group. Without something to compare it to, it is difficult to assess the significance of an observed change in the case. The change could be the result of historical changes unrelated to the treatment, the maturation of the subject, or an artifact of the testing.

Even when pre-experimental designs identify a comparison group, it is still difficult to dismiss rival hypotheses for the observed change. This is because there is no formal way to determine whether the two groups would have been the same if it had not been for the treatment. If the treatment group and the comparison group differ after the treatment, this might be a reflection of differences in the initial recruitment to the groups or differential mortality in the experiment.

Advantages and Disadvantages

As exploratory approaches, pre-experiments can be a cost-effective way to discern whether a potential explanation is worthy of further investigation.

Disadvantages

Pre-experiments offer few advantages since it is often difficult or impossible to rule out alternative explanations. The nearly insurmountable threats to their validity are clearly the most important disadvantage of pre-experimental research designs.

One-Group Posttest Only Design: An Introduction

The one-group posttest-only design (a.k.a. one-shot case study ) is a type of quasi-experiment in which the outcome of interest is measured only once after exposing a non-random group of participants to a certain intervention.

The objective is to evaluate the effect of that intervention which can be:

  • A training program
  • A policy change
  • A medical treatment, etc.

One-group posttest-only design representation

As in other quasi-experiments, the group of participants who receive the intervention is selected in a non-random way (for example according to their choosing or that of the researcher).

The one-group posttest-only design is especially characterized by having:

  • No control group
  • No measurements before the intervention

It is the simplest and weakest of the quasi-experimental designs in terms of level of evidence as the measured outcome cannot be compared to a measurement before the intervention nor to a control group.

So the outcome will be compared to what we assume will happen if the intervention was not implemented. This is generally based on expert knowledge and speculation.

Next we will discuss cases where this design can be useful and its limitations in the study of a causal relationship between the intervention and the outcome.

Advantages and Limitations of the one-group posttest-only design

Advantages of the one-group posttest-only design, 1. advantages related to the non-random selection of participants:.

  • Ethical considerations: Random selection of participants is considered unethical when the intervention is believed to be harmful (for example exposing people to smoking or dangerous chemicals) or on the contrary when it is believed to be so beneficial that it would be malevolent not to offer it to all participants (for example a groundbreaking treatment or medical operation).
  • Difficulty to adequately randomize subjects and locations: In some cases where the intervention acts on a group of people at a given location, it becomes infeasible to adequately randomize subjects (ex. an intervention that reduces pollution in a given area).

2. Advantages related to the simplicity of this design:

  • Feasible with fewer resources than most designs: The one-group posttest-only design is especially useful when the intervention must be quickly introduced and we do not have enough time to take pre-intervention measurements. Other designs may also require a larger sample size or a higher cost to account for the follow-up of a control group.
  • No temporality issue: Since the outcome is measured after the intervention, we can be certain that it occurred after it, which is important for inferring a causal relationship between the two.

Limitations of the one-group posttest-only design

1. selection bias:.

Because participants were not chosen at random, it is certainly possible that those who volunteered are not representative of the population of interest on which we intend to draw our conclusions.

2. Limitation due to maturation:

Because we don’t have a control group nor a pre-intervention measurement of the variable of interest, the post-intervention measurement will be compared to what we believe or assume would happen was there no intervention at all.

The problem is when the outcome of interest has a natural fluctuation pattern (maturation effect) that we don’t know about.

So since certain factors are essentially hard to predict and since 1 measurement is certainly not enough to understand the natural pattern of an outcome, therefore with the one-group posttest-only design, we can hardly infer any causal relationship between intervention and outcome.

3. Limitation due to history:

The idea here is that we may have a historical event, which may also influence the outcome, occurring at the same time as the intervention.

The problem is that this event can now be an alternative explanation of the observed outcome. The only way out of this is if the effect of this event on the outcome is well-known and documented in order to account for it in our data analysis.

This is why most of the time we prefer other designs that include a control group (made of people who were exposed to the historical event but not to the intervention) as it provides us with a reference to compare to.

Example of a study that used the one-group posttest-only design

In 2018, Tsai et al. designed an exercise program for older adults based on traditional Chinese medicine ideas, and wanted to test its feasibility, safety and helpfulness.

So they conducted a one-group posttest-only study as a pilot test with 31 older adult volunteers. Then they evaluated these participants (using open-ended questions) after receiving the intervention (the exercise program).

The study concluded that the program was safe, helpful and suitable for older adults.

What can we learn from this example?

1. work within the design limitations:.

Notice that the outcome measured was the feasibility of the program and not its health effects on older adults.

The purpose of the study was to design an exercise program based on the participants’ feedback. So a pilot one-group posttest-only study was enough to do so.

For studying the health effects of this program on older adults a randomized controlled trial will certainly be necessary.

2. Be careful with generalization when working with non-randomly selected participants:

For instance, participants who volunteered to be in this study were all physically active older adults who exercise regularly.

Therefore, the study results may not generalize to all the elderly population.

  • Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference . 2nd Edition. Cengage Learning; 2001.
  • Campbell DT, Stanley J. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research . 1st Edition. Cengage Learning; 1963.

Further reading

  • Understand Quasi-Experimental Design Through an Example
  • Experimental vs Quasi-Experimental Design
  • Static-Group Comparison Design
  • One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design

The is shown in figure 4.1e. It is also called the because a single group of people is measured on some dependent variable an intervention has taken place.

This is the most common research design in culture change studies, where it is obviously impossible to manipulate the dependent variable. You arrive in a community and notice that something important has taken place. A clinic or a school has been built. You try to evaluate the experiment by interviewing people (O) and assessing the impact of the intervention (X).

With neither a pretest nor a control group, you can’t be sure that what you observe is the result of some particular intervention. Despite this apparent weakness, however, the intuitive appeal of findings produced by one-shot case studies can be formidable.

In the 1950s, physicians began general use of the Pap Test, a simple office procedure for determining the presence of cervical cancer. Figure 4.2 shows that since 1950, the death rate from cervical cancer in the United States has dropped steadily, from about 18

BOX 4.3 POSTTEST ONLY: THE UNDERAPPRECIATED DESIGN

The posttest-only design, with random assignment, is not used as much as I think it should be, despite its elegance and its low cost. In June 2009, a search of PsycINFO turned up 1,110 examples of studies that used the pretest-posttest design, compared to 133 for studies that used the posttest-only design (with or without random assignment). This preference for the classic design is due partly to the appealing-but-mistaken idea that matching participants in experiments on key independent variables (age, ethnicity, etc.) is somehow better than randomly assigning participant to groups, and partly to the nagging suspicion that pretests are essential to the experimental method. That nagging suspicion (that we can do better than trust the outcome of events to randomness) has been the focus of a lot of research since a paper by Gilovich et al. in 1985 titled: ''The hot hand in basketball—on the misperception of random sequences.'' The hot-hand phenomenon—the belief that streaks (in sports and in money management, for example) are the result of nonrandom forces—is hard to break. By the same token, so is the belief that small samples, if drawn randomly, are sufficient to warrant generalizing to a population. On this one, see the 600+ citations to Tversky and Kahneman (1971) and chapters 6 and 7 on representative and nonrepresentative sampling ( the posttest-only design).

per 100,000 women to about 11 in 1970, to about 8.3 in 1980, to about 6.5 in 1995 and to about 2.4 in 2005. If you look only at the data the intervention (the one-shot case study X O design), you could easily conclude that the intervention (the Pap Test) was the sole cause of this drop in cervical cancer deaths. There is no doubt that the continued decline of cervical cancer deaths is due largely to the early detection provided by the Pap Test, but by 1950, the death rate had already declined by 36% from 28 per 100,000 in 1930 (B. Williams 1978:16).

Never use a design of less logical power when one of greater power is feasible. If pretest data are available, use them. On the other hand, a one-shot case study is often the best you can do. Virtually all ethnography falls in this category, and, as I have said before, nothing beats a good story, well told ( case study methods).

one shot case study design research

For the best experience, please use Chrome, Firefox, Safari, or Edge.

Pre-Experimental Designs

Pre-experiments are the simplest form of research design. In a pre-experiment either a single group or multiple groups are observed subsequent to some agent or treatment presumed to cause change.

Types of Pre-Experimental Design

One-shot case study design, one-group pretest-posttest design, static-group comparison.

A single group is studied at a single point in time after some treatment that is presumed to have caused change. The carefully studied single instance is compared to general expectations of what the case would have looked like had the treatment not occurred and to other events casually observed. No control or comparison group is employed.

A single case is observed at two time points, one before the treatment and one after the treatment. Changes in the outcome of interest are presumed to be the result of the intervention or treatment. No control or comparison group is employed.

A group that has experienced some treatment is compared with one that has not. Observed differences between the two groups are assumed to be a result of the treatment.

Validity of Results

An important drawback of pre-experimental designs is that they are subject to numerous threats to their validity . Consequently, it is often difficult or impossible to dismiss rival hypotheses or explanations. Therefore, researchers must exercise extreme caution in interpreting and generalizing the results from pre-experimental studies.

One reason that it is often difficult to assess the validity of studies that employ a pre-experimental design is that they often do not include any control or comparison group. Without something to compare it to, it is difficult to assess the significance of an observed change in the case. The change could be the result of historical changes unrelated to the treatment, the maturation of the subject, or an artifact of the testing.

Even when pre-experimental designs identify a comparison group, it is still difficult to dismiss rival hypotheses for the observed change. This is because there is no formal way to determine whether the two groups would have been the same if it had not been for the treatment. If the treatment group and the comparison group differ after the treatment, this might be a reflection of differences in the initial recruitment to the groups or differential mortality in the experiment.

Advantages and Disadvantages

As exploratory approaches, pre-experiments can be a cost-effective way to discern whether a potential explanation is worthy of further investigation.

Disadvantages

Pre-experiments offer few advantages since it is often difficult or impossible to rule out alternative explanations. The nearly insurmountable threats to their validity are clearly the most important disadvantage of pre-experimental research designs.

Warning: The NCBI web site requires JavaScript to function. more...

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Ip S, Paulus JK, Balk EM, et al. Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Comparative Effectiveness Reviews [Internet]. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2013 Jan.

Cover of Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Comparative Effectiveness Reviews

Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Comparative Effectiveness Reviews [Internet].

Evidence from randomized controlled trials is often unavailable or insufficient to answer all questions posed in a comparative effectiveness review (CER). Thus, following a best-available-evidence approach, 1 systematic reviewers often use observational studies including a comparison group to examine the comparative effectiveness and safety of alternative therapeutic strategies. However, there are many instances where even observational studies with a comparison group are unavailable. Therefore, single group studies—those that evaluate a single intervention given to all subjects included in the study design—are often part of the evidence available to systematic reviewers conducting CERs.

We define a single group study as a study that consists of only a single group of subjects included in the study design, in which all subjects received a single intervention and the outcomes are assessed over time (i.e., not a cross-sectional study). These studies may be prospective or retrospective cohort studies. A number of study types would be included in this category, including investigations described as “single arm studies,” case series, registries, “before-after designs,” and time series studies. A classification scheme developed by Campbell and Stanley describes two single group studies consistent with our definition: the “one-shot case study” and the “one-group pretest–post-test design.” 2 In the one-shot case study, a single group is studied only once after a treatment is applied. In the one-group pretest–post-test design, a pretest evaluation is followed by a treatment and then a post-test. For the rest of this paper, we will use the simplified term “single group study” when describing these designs in general.

Single group studies are often conducted in the setting of strong therapy preferences (e.g., hyperbaric oxygen therapy for arterial gas embolism 3 ). This is especially true for transplantation studies of vital organs in the setting of rapid and fatal disease progression. For example, in patients with end-stage liver disease, the natural history of disease is so well known that it would be difficult to carry out a trial with an untransplanted study arm. Also, a field of clinical inquiry that is relatively new may not be sufficiently mature to rationalize a comparative hypothesis. For example, novel procedures or drugs are often initially evaluated in single group studies that are used to inform the design of a subsequent study with an internal comparison group.

Single group study designs are commonly used to monitor adverse events that may become evident only with long-term followup of large numbers of treated patients, which is not practical or efficient with other study designs. For example, phase 4 studies to monitor postmarketing adverse events and evaluations of therapies often include a single group of patients managed with the same treatment strategy and followed over time. Open-label extensions of clinical trials present another type of clinical investigation that often lacks an internal, concurrent comparison group. Although they are designed to follow patients for an extended period of time, they also usually evaluate a more highly selected population of patients who completed the randomized trial, tolerated the medication, and agreed to participate in the extension. Expanded access programs (or “compassionate use”) allow the use of an investigational drug outside of a clinical trial to treat a patient with a serious or immediately life-threatening disease or condition lacking satisfactory alternative treatment options. These investigations commonly describe the experience of a single group of patients without a comparison group (for example, see Janne 2004 4 ). Finally, registries of patients who have been exposed to a single drug or device may also be assembled for monitoring long-term sequelae without an internal comparison group. An example includes the coordinated effort to study newly introduced devices through the Interagency Registry for Mechanically Assisted Circulatory Support, established to capture detailed clinical data on all patients receiving implantable ventricular assist pumps in the United States. 5

Since single group studies do not include a direct, concurrent comparison group, their role in informing comparative effectiveness questions is not straightforward. Observational study designs in general suffer from a potential lack of exchangeability of exposed and unexposed subjects. In other words, the outcome in the untreated group may differ from what would have occurred in the treated in the absence of treatment (the “counterfactual outcome”). The absence of a direct, concurrent untreated comparator in single group studies presents an added challenge to identifying a proxy for the counterfactual, or an answer to the question: “What would have been the treated person's experience if there had been no treatment?” Extrapolations based on the expected outcomes in the “missing” untreated arm are required for inference about treatment effects. In fact, explicit and implicit comparisons are frequently made in single group studies even in the absence of a direct, concurrent comparator. The appropriate interpretation of these implicit and explicit comparisons and their potential utility in CERs must include consideration of the key assumptions underlying each single group design.

The ability of observational studies to answer questions about the benefits or intended effects of pharmacotherapeutic agents, devices, or procedural interventions has been a matter of debate. 6 Guidance has been developed for systematic reviewers for decisionmaking on the inclusion of observational studies in general in CERs. 6 However, to the best of our knowledge, the use of single group observational studies in CERs has not been specifically addressed in this methods guide or elsewhere. While the value of using single group studies to identify and quantify the occurrence of harms a of interventions is well recognized, the role of these studies in evaluating comparative effectiveness and safety is not well developed. Given that single group studies may comprise a substantial portion of the evidence base for a given clinical question, and in light of the challenges in their interpretation and relevance to questions that are comparative in nature, it is important to clarify whether they are useful in informing comparative effectiveness assessments, and if so, to clarify the assumptions required to support their use.

In order to illuminate the use of single group studies in CERs, we conducted an empirical review of current practices in using single group studies in CERs conducted by Evidence-based Practice Centers (EPCs) for the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ). The summary findings should serve as an impetus for future work in reaching a consensus across EPCs as to when and how single group studies should be used in CERs specifically and systematic reviews in general. In addition to the empirical review, we also provided a narrative review section describing the common single group study designs and the key considerations and assumptions required for their interpretation to help guide comparative effectiveness reviewers who encounter this type of evidence.

Includes adverse events of interventions as well as other harmful events that may be indirectly related to the intervention.

  • Cite this Page Ip S, Paulus JK, Balk EM, et al. Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Comparative Effectiveness Reviews [Internet]. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2013 Jan. Background.
  • PDF version of this title (544K)

Other titles in these collections

  • AHRQ Methods for Effective Health Care
  • Health Services/Technology Assessment Texts (HSTAT)

Recent Activity

  • Background - Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and ... Background - Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Comparative Effectiveness Reviews

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

psychology

Pre-Experimental Design

Pre-experimental design refers to the simplest form of research design often used in the field of psychology, sociology, education, and other social sciences. These designs are called “pre-experimental” because they precede true experimental design in terms of complexity and rigor.

In pre-experimental designs, researchers observe or measure subjects without manipulating variables or controlling conditions. Often, these designs lack certain elements of a true experiment, such as random assignment, control groups, or pretest measurements, making it difficult to determine causality.

Three common types of pre-experimental designs include the one-shot case study, the one-group pretest-posttest design, and the static-group comparison. These designs offer a starting point for researchers but are typically seen as less reliable than more controlled experimental designs due to the lack of randomization and the potential for confounding variables.

Characteristics of Pre-Experimental Design

Pre-experimental designs are characterized by their simplicity and ease of execution. They are typically used when resources are limited, or when the research question does not require a high degree of control or precision. Key characteristics of these designs include the use of a single group, the lack of a control group, and the absence of random assignment.

Single Group

In a pre-experimental design, there is typically only one group of subjects, and this group is measured or observed both before and after an intervention or treatment.

Lack of Control Group

Pre-experimental designs often lack a control group for comparison. As a result, it’s difficult to determine whether observed changes are the result of the intervention or due to extraneous factors.

Absence of Random Assignment

Another characteristic of pre-experimental design is the absence of random assignment. Subjects are not randomly assigned to groups, which can lead to selection bias and limits the generalizability of the findings.

There are several types of pre-experimental designs, including the one-shot case study, the one-group pretest-posttest design, and the static-group comparison.

One-Shot Case Study

In a one-shot case study, a single group or case is studied at a single point in time after some intervention or treatment that is presumed to cause change.

One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design

In the one-group pretest-posttest design, a single group is observed at two time points, one before the treatment and one after the treatment.

Static-Group Comparison

In a static-group comparison, there are two groups that are not created through random assignment. One group receives the treatment and the other does not, and the outcomes are compared.

Limitations

While pre-experimental designs offer advantages in terms of simplicity and convenience, they also come with notable limitations. The lack of a control group and the absence of random assignment limits the ability to establish causality. There is also a risk of selection bias, and the findings may not be generalizable to other populations or settings.

Despite these limitations, pre-experimental designs can serve as valuable starting points in exploratory research, laying the groundwork for more rigorous experimental designs in the future.

In conclusion, pre-experimental design, while limited in its ability to provide strong evidence of causality, plays a crucial role in exploratory research. It presents a simplified and cost-effective approach to experimentation that is especially useful when resources are limited or when the goal is to explore a new area of study. However, the inherent limitations of pre-experimental designs necessitate caution in interpreting their results. Consequently, they are often used as stepping stones towards more rigorous research designs. As such, understanding pre-experimental designs is a fundamental part of the researcher’s toolkit, paving the way for more comprehensive and controlled investigations.

Our websites may use cookies to personalize and enhance your experience. By continuing without changing your cookie settings, you agree to this collection. For more information, please see our University Websites Privacy Notice .

Neag School of Education

Educational Research Basics by Del Siegle

Experimental research designs.

R=Random Assignment X= Treatment O=Observation (Assessment)

X O One Shot Case Study Design
O X O One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design
X O Static-Group Comparison Design
O
O X O Static-Group Pretest-Posttest Design
O O
R X O Randomized Posttest-Only, Control Group Design
R O
R O X O Randomized Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
R O O
R O X O Ranomized Solomon Four-Group Design
R O O
R X O
R O

R = Random Assignment X = Treatment O = Observation (test or measurement of some type)

Del Siegle, Ph.D. Neag School of Education – University of Connecticut [email protected] www.delsiegle.com

  • Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

busayo.longe

Experimental research is the most familiar type of research design for individuals in the physical sciences and a host of other fields. This is mainly because experimental research is a classical scientific experiment, similar to those performed in high school science classes.

Imagine taking 2 samples of the same plant and exposing one of them to sunlight, while the other is kept away from sunlight. Let the plant exposed to sunlight be called sample A, while the latter is called sample B.

If after the duration of the research, we find out that sample A grows and sample B dies, even though they are both regularly wetted and given the same treatment. Therefore, we can conclude that sunlight will aid growth in all similar plants.

What is Experimental Research?

Experimental research is a scientific approach to research, where one or more independent variables are manipulated and applied to one or more dependent variables to measure their effect on the latter. The effect of the independent variables on the dependent variables is usually observed and recorded over some time, to aid researchers in drawing a reasonable conclusion regarding the relationship between these 2 variable types.

The experimental research method is widely used in physical and social sciences, psychology, and education. It is based on the comparison between two or more groups with a straightforward logic, which may, however, be difficult to execute.

Mostly related to a laboratory test procedure, experimental research designs involve collecting quantitative data and performing statistical analysis on them during research. Therefore, making it an example of quantitative research method .

What are The Types of Experimental Research Design?

The types of experimental research design are determined by the way the researcher assigns subjects to different conditions and groups. They are of 3 types, namely; pre-experimental, quasi-experimental, and true experimental research.

Pre-experimental Research Design

In pre-experimental research design, either a group or various dependent groups are observed for the effect of the application of an independent variable which is presumed to cause change. It is the simplest form of experimental research design and is treated with no control group.

Although very practical, experimental research is lacking in several areas of the true-experimental criteria. The pre-experimental research design is further divided into three types

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design

In this type of experimental study, only one dependent group or variable is considered. The study is carried out after some treatment which was presumed to cause change, making it a posttest study.

  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design: 

This research design combines both posttest and pretest study by carrying out a test on a single group before the treatment is administered and after the treatment is administered. With the former being administered at the beginning of treatment and later at the end.

  • Static-group Comparison: 

In a static-group comparison study, 2 or more groups are placed under observation, where only one of the groups is subjected to some treatment while the other groups are held static. All the groups are post-tested, and the observed differences between the groups are assumed to be a result of the treatment.

Quasi-experimental Research Design

  The word “quasi” means partial, half, or pseudo. Therefore, the quasi-experimental research bearing a resemblance to the true experimental research, but not the same.  In quasi-experiments, the participants are not randomly assigned, and as such, they are used in settings where randomization is difficult or impossible.

 This is very common in educational research, where administrators are unwilling to allow the random selection of students for experimental samples.

Some examples of quasi-experimental research design include; the time series, no equivalent control group design, and the counterbalanced design.

True Experimental Research Design

The true experimental research design relies on statistical analysis to approve or disprove a hypothesis. It is the most accurate type of experimental design and may be carried out with or without a pretest on at least 2 randomly assigned dependent subjects.

The true experimental research design must contain a control group, a variable that can be manipulated by the researcher, and the distribution must be random. The classification of true experimental design include:

  • The posttest-only Control Group Design: In this design, subjects are randomly selected and assigned to the 2 groups (control and experimental), and only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested, and a conclusion is drawn from the difference between these groups.
  • The pretest-posttest Control Group Design: For this control group design, subjects are randomly assigned to the 2 groups, both are presented, but only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested to measure the degree of change in each group.
  • Solomon four-group Design: This is the combination of the pretest-only and the pretest-posttest control groups. In this case, the randomly selected subjects are placed into 4 groups.

The first two of these groups are tested using the posttest-only method, while the other two are tested using the pretest-posttest method.

Examples of Experimental Research

Experimental research examples are different, depending on the type of experimental research design that is being considered. The most basic example of experimental research is laboratory experiments, which may differ in nature depending on the subject of research.

Administering Exams After The End of Semester

During the semester, students in a class are lectured on particular courses and an exam is administered at the end of the semester. In this case, the students are the subjects or dependent variables while the lectures are the independent variables treated on the subjects.

Only one group of carefully selected subjects are considered in this research, making it a pre-experimental research design example. We will also notice that tests are only carried out at the end of the semester, and not at the beginning.

Further making it easy for us to conclude that it is a one-shot case study research. 

Employee Skill Evaluation

Before employing a job seeker, organizations conduct tests that are used to screen out less qualified candidates from the pool of qualified applicants. This way, organizations can determine an employee’s skill set at the point of employment.

In the course of employment, organizations also carry out employee training to improve employee productivity and generally grow the organization. Further evaluation is carried out at the end of each training to test the impact of the training on employee skills, and test for improvement.

Here, the subject is the employee, while the treatment is the training conducted. This is a pretest-posttest control group experimental research example.

Evaluation of Teaching Method

Let us consider an academic institution that wants to evaluate the teaching method of 2 teachers to determine which is best. Imagine a case whereby the students assigned to each teacher is carefully selected probably due to personal request by parents or due to stubbornness and smartness.

This is a no equivalent group design example because the samples are not equal. By evaluating the effectiveness of each teacher’s teaching method this way, we may conclude after a post-test has been carried out.

However, this may be influenced by factors like the natural sweetness of a student. For example, a very smart student will grab more easily than his or her peers irrespective of the method of teaching.

What are the Characteristics of Experimental Research?  

Experimental research contains dependent, independent and extraneous variables. The dependent variables are the variables being treated or manipulated and are sometimes called the subject of the research.

The independent variables are the experimental treatment being exerted on the dependent variables. Extraneous variables, on the other hand, are other factors affecting the experiment that may also contribute to the change.

The setting is where the experiment is carried out. Many experiments are carried out in the laboratory, where control can be exerted on the extraneous variables, thereby eliminating them. 

Other experiments are carried out in a less controllable setting. The choice of setting used in research depends on the nature of the experiment being carried out.

  • Multivariable

Experimental research may include multiple independent variables, e.g. time, skills, test scores, etc.

Why Use Experimental Research Design?  

Experimental research design can be majorly used in physical sciences, social sciences, education, and psychology. It is used to make predictions and draw conclusions on a subject matter. 

Some uses of experimental research design are highlighted below.

  • Medicine: Experimental research is used to provide the proper treatment for diseases. In most cases, rather than directly using patients as the research subject, researchers take a sample of the bacteria from the patient’s body and are treated with the developed antibacterial

The changes observed during this period are recorded and evaluated to determine its effectiveness. This process can be carried out using different experimental research methods.

  • Education: Asides from science subjects like Chemistry and Physics which involves teaching students how to perform experimental research, it can also be used in improving the standard of an academic institution. This includes testing students’ knowledge on different topics, coming up with better teaching methods, and the implementation of other programs that will aid student learning.
  • Human Behavior: Social scientists are the ones who mostly use experimental research to test human behaviour. For example, consider 2 people randomly chosen to be the subject of the social interaction research where one person is placed in a room without human interaction for 1 year.

The other person is placed in a room with a few other people, enjoying human interaction. There will be a difference in their behaviour at the end of the experiment.

  • UI/UX: During the product development phase, one of the major aims of the product team is to create a great user experience with the product. Therefore, before launching the final product design, potential are brought in to interact with the product.

For example, when finding it difficult to choose how to position a button or feature on the app interface, a random sample of product testers are allowed to test the 2 samples and how the button positioning influences the user interaction is recorded.

What are the Disadvantages of Experimental Research?  

  • It is highly prone to human error due to its dependency on variable control which may not be properly implemented. These errors could eliminate the validity of the experiment and the research being conducted.
  • Exerting control of extraneous variables may create unrealistic situations. Eliminating real-life variables will result in inaccurate conclusions. This may also result in researchers controlling the variables to suit his or her personal preferences.
  • It is a time-consuming process. So much time is spent on testing dependent variables and waiting for the effect of the manipulation of dependent variables to manifest.
  • It is expensive. 
  • It is very risky and may have ethical complications that cannot be ignored. This is common in medical research, where failed trials may lead to a patient’s death or a deteriorating health condition.
  • Experimental research results are not descriptive.
  • Response bias can also be supplied by the subject of the conversation.
  • Human responses in experimental research can be difficult to measure. 

What are the Data Collection Methods in Experimental Research?  

Data collection methods in experimental research are the different ways in which data can be collected for experimental research. They are used in different cases, depending on the type of research being carried out.

1. Observational Study

This type of study is carried out over a long period. It measures and observes the variables of interest without changing existing conditions.

When researching the effect of social interaction on human behavior, the subjects who are placed in 2 different environments are observed throughout the research. No matter the kind of absurd behavior that is exhibited by the subject during this period, its condition will not be changed.

This may be a very risky thing to do in medical cases because it may lead to death or worse medical conditions.

2. Simulations

This procedure uses mathematical, physical, or computer models to replicate a real-life process or situation. It is frequently used when the actual situation is too expensive, dangerous, or impractical to replicate in real life.

This method is commonly used in engineering and operational research for learning purposes and sometimes as a tool to estimate possible outcomes of real research. Some common situation software are Simulink, MATLAB, and Simul8.

Not all kinds of experimental research can be carried out using simulation as a data collection tool . It is very impractical for a lot of laboratory-based research that involves chemical processes.

A survey is a tool used to gather relevant data about the characteristics of a population and is one of the most common data collection tools. A survey consists of a group of questions prepared by the researcher, to be answered by the research subject.

Surveys can be shared with the respondents both physically and electronically. When collecting data through surveys, the kind of data collected depends on the respondent, and researchers have limited control over it.

Formplus is the best tool for collecting experimental data using survey s. It has relevant features that will aid the data collection process and can also be used in other aspects of experimental research.

Differences between Experimental and Non-Experimental Research 

1. In experimental research, the researcher can control and manipulate the environment of the research, including the predictor variable which can be changed. On the other hand, non-experimental research cannot be controlled or manipulated by the researcher at will.

This is because it takes place in a real-life setting, where extraneous variables cannot be eliminated. Therefore, it is more difficult to conclude non-experimental studies, even though they are much more flexible and allow for a greater range of study fields.

2. The relationship between cause and effect cannot be established in non-experimental research, while it can be established in experimental research. This may be because many extraneous variables also influence the changes in the research subject, making it difficult to point at a particular variable as the cause of a particular change

3. Independent variables are not introduced, withdrawn, or manipulated in non-experimental designs, but the same may not be said about experimental research.

Conclusion  

Experimental research designs are often considered to be the standard in research designs. This is partly due to the common misconception that research is equivalent to scientific experiments—a component of experimental research design.

In this research design, one or more subjects or dependent variables are randomly assigned to different treatments (i.e. independent variables manipulated by the researcher) and the results are observed to conclude. One of the uniqueness of experimental research is in its ability to control the effect of extraneous variables.

Experimental research is suitable for research whose goal is to examine cause-effect relationships, e.g. explanatory research. It can be conducted in the laboratory or field settings, depending on the aim of the research that is being carried out. 

Logo

Connect to Formplus, Get Started Now - It's Free!

  • examples of experimental research
  • experimental research methods
  • types of experimental research
  • busayo.longe

Formplus

You may also like:

Experimental Vs Non-Experimental Research: 15 Key Differences

Differences between experimental and non experimental research on definitions, types, examples, data collection tools, uses, advantages etc.

one shot case study design research

Simpson’s Paradox & How to Avoid it in Experimental Research

In this article, we are going to look at Simpson’s Paradox from its historical point and later, we’ll consider its effect in...

What is Experimenter Bias? Definition, Types & Mitigation

In this article, we will look into the concept of experimental bias and how it can be identified in your research

Response vs Explanatory Variables: Definition & Examples

In this article, we’ll be comparing the two types of variables, what they both mean and see some of their real-life applications in research

Formplus - For Seamless Data Collection

Collect data the right way with a versatile data collection tool. try formplus and transform your work productivity today..

one shot case study design research

  • Voxco Online
  • Voxco Panel Management
  • Voxco Panel Portal
  • Voxco Audience
  • Voxco Mobile Offline
  • Voxco Dialer Cloud
  • Voxco Dialer On-premise
  • Voxco TCPA Connect
  • Voxco Analytics
  • Voxco Text & Sentiment Analysis

one shot case study design research

  • 40+ question types
  • Drag-and-drop interface
  • Skip logic and branching
  • Multi-lingual survey
  • Text piping
  • Question library
  • CSS customization
  • White-label surveys
  • Customizable ‘Thank You’ page
  • Customizable survey theme
  • Reminder send-outs
  • Survey rewards
  • Social media
  • Website surveys
  • Correlation analysis
  • Cross-tabulation analysis
  • Trend analysis
  • Real-time dashboard
  • Customizable report
  • Email address validation
  • Recaptcha validation
  • SSL security

Take a peek at our powerful survey features to design surveys that scale discoveries.

Download feature sheet.

  • Hospitality
  • Financial Services
  • Academic Research
  • Customer Experience
  • Employee Experience
  • Product Experience
  • Market Research
  • Social Research
  • Data Analysis

Explore Voxco 

Need to map Voxco’s features & offerings? We can help!

Watch a Demo 

Download Brochures 

Get a Quote

  • NPS Calculator
  • CES Calculator
  • A/B Testing Calculator
  • Margin of Error Calculator
  • Sample Size Calculator
  • CX Strategy & Management Hub
  • Market Research Hub
  • Patient Experience Hub
  • Employee Experience Hub
  • NPS Knowledge Hub
  • Market Research Guide
  • Customer Experience Guide
  • The Voxco Guide to Customer Experience
  • Survey Research Guides
  • Survey Template Library
  • Webinars and Events
  • Feature Sheets
  • Try a sample survey
  • Professional services

Find the best customer experience platform

Uncover customer pain points, analyze feedback and run successful CX programs with the best CX platform for your team.

Get the Guide Now

one shot case study design research

We’ve been avid users of the Voxco platform now for over 20 years. It gives us the flexibility to routinely enhance our survey toolkit and provides our clients with a more robust dataset and story to tell their clients.

VP Innovation & Strategic Partnerships, The Logit Group

  • Client Stories
  • Voxco Reviews
  • Why Voxco Research?
  • Careers at Voxco
  • Vulnerabilities and Ethical Hacking

Explore Regional Offices

  • Survey Software The world’s leading omnichannel survey software
  • Online Survey Tools Create sophisticated surveys with ease.
  • Mobile Offline Conduct efficient field surveys.
  • Text Analysis
  • Close The Loop
  • Automated Translations
  • NPS Dashboard
  • CATI Manage high volume phone surveys efficiently
  • Cloud/On-premise Dialer TCPA compliant Cloud on-premise dialer
  • IVR Survey Software Boost productivity with automated call workflows.
  • Analytics Analyze survey data with visual dashboards
  • Panel Manager Nurture a loyal community of respondents.
  • Survey Portal Best-in-class user friendly survey portal.
  • Voxco Audience Conduct targeted sample research in hours.
  • Predictive Analytics
  • Customer 360
  • Customer Loyalty
  • Fraud & Risk Management
  • AI/ML Enablement Services
  • Credit Underwriting

one shot case study design research

Find the best survey software for you! (Along with a checklist to compare platforms)

Get Buyer’s Guide

  • 100+ question types
  • SMS surveys
  • Banking & Financial Services
  • Retail Solution
  • Risk Management
  • Customer Lifecycle Solutions
  • Net Promoter Score
  • Customer Behaviour Analytics
  • Customer Segmentation
  • Data Unification

Explore Voxco 

Watch a Demo 

Download Brochures 

  • CX Strategy & Management Hub
  • Blogs & White papers
  • Case Studies

one shot case study design research

VP Innovation & Strategic Partnerships, The Logit Group

  • Why Voxco Intelligence?
  • Our clients
  • Client stories
  • Featuresheets

Pre experimental design1

Pre-experimental Design: Definition, Types & Examples

  • October 1, 2021

SHARE THE ARTICLE ON

Experimental research is conducted to analyze and understand the effect of a program or a treatment. There are three types of experimental research designs – pre-experimental designs, true experimental designs, and quasi-experimental designs . 

In this blog, we will be talking about pre-experimental designs. Let’s first explain pre-experimental research. 

What is Pre-experimental Research?

As the name suggests, pre- experimental research happens even before the true experiment starts. This is done to determine the researchers’ intervention on a group of people. This will help them tell if the investment of cost and time for conducting a true experiment is worth a while. Hence, pre-experimental research is a preliminary step to justify the presence of the researcher’s intervention. 

The pre-experimental approach helps give some sort of guarantee that the experiment can be a full-scale successful study. 

What is Pre-experimental Design?

The pre-experimental design includes one or more than one experimental groups to be observed against certain treatments. It is the simplest form of research design that follows the basic steps in experiments. 

The pre-experimental design does not have a comparison group. This means that while a researcher can claim that participants who received certain treatment have experienced a change, they cannot conclude that the change was caused by the treatment itself. 

The research design can still be useful for exploratory research to test the feasibility for further study. 

Let us understand how pre-experimental design is different from the true and quasi-experiments:

Pre experimental design2

The above table tells us pretty much about the working of the pre-experimental designs. So we can say that it is actually to test treatment, and check whether it has the potential to cause a change or not. For the same reasons, it is advised to perform pre-experiments to define the potential of a true experiment.

See Voxco survey software in action with a Free demo.

Types of Pre-experimental Designs

Assuming now you have a better understanding of what the whole pre-experimental design concept is, it is time to move forward and look at its types and their working:

One-shot case study design

  • This design practices the treatment of a single group.
  • It only takes a single measurement after the experiment.
  • A one-shot case study design only analyses post-test results.

Pre experimental design3

The one-shot case study compares the post-test results to the expected results. It makes clear what the result is and how the case would have looked if the treatment wasn’t done. 

A team leader wants to implement a new soft skills program in the firm. The employees can be measured at the end of the first month to see the improvement in their soft skills. The team leader will know the impact of the program on the employees.

One-group pretest-posttest design

  • Like the previous one, this design also works on just one experimental group.
  • But this one takes two measures into account. 
  • A pre-test and a post-test are conducted. 

Pre experimental design4

As the name suggests, it includes one group and conducts pre-test and post-test on it. The pre-test will tell how the group was before they were put under treatment. Whereas post-test determines the changes in the group after the treatment. 

This sounds like a true experiment , but being a pre-experiment design, it does not have any control group. 

Following the previous example, the team leader here will conduct two tests. One before the soft skill program implementation to know the level of employees before they were put through the training. And a post-test to know their status after the training.

Now that he has a frame of reference, he knows exactly how the program helped the employees. 

Static-group comparison

  • This compares two experimental groups.
  • One group is exposed to the treatment.
  • The other group is not exposed to the treatment.
  • The difference between the two groups is the result of the experiment.

Pre experimental design5

As the name suggests, it has two groups, which means it involves a control group too. 

In static-group comparison design, the two groups are observed as one goes through the treatment while the other does not. They are then compared to each other to determine the outcome of the treatment.

The team lead decides one group of employees to get the soft skills training while the other group remains as a control group and is not exposed to any program. He then compares both the groups and finds out the treatment group has evolved in their soft skills more than the control group. 

Due to such working, static-group comparison design is generally perceived as a quasi-experimental design too. 

Customer experience

Characteristics of Pre-experimental Designs

In this section, let us point down the characteristics of pre-experimental design:

  • Generally uses only one group for treatment which makes observation simple and easy.
  • Validates the experiment in the preliminary phase itself. 
  • Pre-experimental design tells the researchers how their intervention will affect the whole study. 
  • As they are conducted in the beginning, pre-experimental designs give evidence for or against their intervention.
  • It does not involve the randomization of the participants. 
  • It generally does not involve the control group, but in some cases where there is a need for studying the control group against the treatment group, static-group comparison comes into the picture. 
  • The pre-experimental design gives an idea about how the treatment is going to work in case of actual true experiments.  

Validity of results in Pre-experimental Designs

Validity means a level to which data or results reflect the accuracy of reality. And in the case of pre-experimental research design, it is a tough catch. The reason being testing a hypothesis or dissolving a problem can be quite a difficult task, let’s say close to impossible. This being said, researchers find it challenging to generalize the results they got from the pre-experimental design, over the actual experiment. 

As pre-experimental design generally does not have any comparison groups to compete for the results with, that makes it pretty obvious for the researchers to go through the trouble of believing its results. Without comparison, it is hard to tell how significant or valid the result is. Because there is a chance that the result occurred due to some uncalled changes in the treatment, maturation of the group, or is it just sheer chance. 

Let’s say all the above parameters work just in favor of your experiment, you even have a control group to compare it with, but that still leaves us with one problem. And that is what “kind” of groups we get for the true experiments. It is possible that the subjects in your pre-experimental design were a lot different from the subjects you have for the true experiment. If this is the case, even if your treatment is constant, there is still going to be a change in your results. 

Advantages of Pre-experimental Designs

  • Cost-effective due to its easy process. 
  • Very simple to conduct.
  • Efficient to conduct in the natural environment. 
  • It is also suitable for beginners. 
  • Involves less human intervention. 
  • Determines how your treatment is going to affect the true experiment. 

Disadvantages of Pre-experimental Designs

  • It is a weak design to determine causal relationships between variables. 
  • Does not have any control over the research. 
  • Possess a high threat to internal validity. 
  • Researchers find it tough to examine the results’ integrity. 
  • The absence of a control group makes the results less reliable. 

This sums up the basics of pre-experimental design and how it differs from other experimental research designs. Curious to learn how you can use survey software to conduct your experimental research, book a meeting with us . 

Explore all the survey question types possible on Voxco

Pre-experimental design is a research method that happens before the true experiment and determines how the researcher’s intervention will affect the experiment.

An example of a pre-experimental design would be a gym trainer implementing a new training schedule for a trainee.

Characteristics of pre-experimental design include its ability to determine the significance of treatment even before the true experiment is performed.

Researchers want to know how their intervention is going to affect the experiment. So even before the true experiment starts, they carry out a pre-experimental research design to determine the possible results of the true experiment.

The pre-experimental design deals with the treatment’s effect on the experiment and is carried out even before the true experiment takes place. While a true experiment is an actual experiment, it is important to conduct its pre-experiment first to see how the intervention is going to affect the experiment.

The true experimental design carries out the pre-test and post-test on both the treatment group as well as a control group. whereas in pre-experimental design, control group and pre-test are options. it does not always have the presence of those two and helps the researcher determine how the real experiment is going to happen.

The main difference between a pre-experimental design and a quasi-experimental design is that pre-experimental design does not use control groups and quasi-experimental design does. Quasi always makes use of the pre-test post-test model of result comparison while pre-experimental design mostly doesn’t.

Non-experimental research methods majorly fall into three categories namely: Cross-sectional research, correlational research and observational research.

Explore Voxco Survey Software

Online page new product image3 02.png 1

+ Omnichannel Survey Software 

+ Online Survey Software 

+ CATI Survey Software 

+ IVR Survey Software 

+ Market Research Tool

+ Customer Experience Tool 

+ Product Experience Software 

+ Enterprise Survey Software 

research 1

Questionnaire

Questionnaire : Types, Examples & How to design Try a free Voxco Online sample survey! Unlock your Sample Survey SHARE THE ARTICLE ON When conducting

Mistakes to avoid when designing your Survey1

9 Mistakes to Avoid While Adding Survey Questions

9 Mistakes to Avoid While Adding Survey Questions See what question types are possible with a sample survey! Try a Sample Survey Table of Contents

anita qa feature 400x250 1

Q&A With Voxco’s Product Manager: Anita Butani

This week, we sat down with our colleague, Product Manager, Anita Butani to learn more about her perspective on Voxco’s product offerings and future directions

What is customer 360?

What is customer 360 ? SHARE THE ARTICLE ON Table of Contents What do you mean by customer 360? Customer 360 or customer 360 view, is

Pre-experimental design: Definition, types & examples Questionnaire

Maximize Data Accuracy and Completeness with Survey Logic

Maximize Data Accuracy and Completeness with Survey Logic SHARE THE ARTICLE ON Table of Contents In today’s data-driven business world, accurate and complete data plays

Interval Scales

How to Appropriately use Interval Scales in your Survey Questions?

How to appropriately use interval scales in your survey questions? See what question types are possible with a sample survey! Try a Sample Survey Table

We use cookies in our website to give you the best browsing experience and to tailor advertising. By continuing to use our website, you give us consent to the use of cookies. Read More

Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
hubspotutk www.voxco.com HubSpot functional cookie. 1 year HTTP
lhc_dir_locale amplifyreach.com --- 52 years ---
lhc_dirclass amplifyreach.com --- 52 years ---
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_fbp www.voxco.com Facebook Pixel advertising first-party cookie 3 months HTTP
__hstc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. 1 year HTTP
__hssrc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. 52 years HTTP
__hssc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. Session HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_gid www.voxco.com Google Universal Analytics short-time unique user tracking identifier. 1 days HTTP
MUID bing.com Microsoft User Identifier tracking cookie used by Bing Ads. 1 year HTTP
MR bat.bing.com Microsoft User Identifier tracking cookie used by Bing Ads. 7 days HTTP
IDE doubleclick.net Google advertising cookie used for user tracking and ad targeting purposes. 2 years HTTP
_vwo_uuid_v2 www.voxco.com Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) user tracking cookie. 1 year HTTP
_vis_opt_s www.voxco.com Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) user tracking cookie that detects if the user is new or returning to a particular campaign. 3 months HTTP
_vis_opt_test_cookie www.voxco.com A session (temporary) cookie used by Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) to detect if the cookies are enabled on the browser of the user or not. 52 years HTTP
_ga www.voxco.com Google Universal Analytics long-time unique user tracking identifier. 2 years HTTP
_uetsid www.voxco.com Microsoft Bing Ads Universal Event Tracking (UET) tracking cookie. 1 days HTTP
vuid vimeo.com Vimeo tracking cookie 2 years HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
__cf_bm hubspot.com Generic CloudFlare functional cookie. Session HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_gcl_au www.voxco.com --- 3 months ---
_gat_gtag_UA_3262734_1 www.voxco.com --- Session ---
_clck www.voxco.com --- 1 year ---
_ga_HNFQQ528PZ www.voxco.com --- 2 years ---
_clsk www.voxco.com --- 1 days ---
visitor_id18452 pardot.com --- 10 years ---
visitor_id18452-hash pardot.com --- 10 years ---
lpv18452 pi.pardot.com --- Session ---
lhc_per www.voxco.com --- 6 months ---
_uetvid www.voxco.com --- 1 year ---

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government, Department of Justice.

Here's how you know

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock A locked padlock ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

NCJRS Virtual Library

Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research, additional details.

437 Madison Avenue , New York , NY 10036 , United States

Box 7600 , Chicago , IL 60680 , United States

No download available

Availability, related topics.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

2. The “One Shot” Case Study Revisited

Profile image of Gary King

Method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.———.(2000). Fuzzy Set Social Science. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Ragin, Charles C. and Howard Becker, eds.(1992). What Is a Case? Exploring the Foundations of Social Inquiry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Savolainen, Jukka.(1994).“The Rationality of Drawing Big Conclusions Based on Small Samples: In Defense of Mill's Methods.” Social Forces 72 (2), 1217–1224. Stinchcombe, Arthur.(1968). Constructing Social Theories.

Related Papers

Papers Revista De Sociologia

Mihaela Vancea

one shot case study design research

Journal of Politics

Bear Braumoeller

Papers: revista de sociologia

Erasmus Journal for Philosophy and Economics

Attilia Ruzzene

Mohammed Faruque Uddin

This article exemplifies Yager’s theory of fuzzy logic for interpersonal communication to the area of social research. Taking the dilemma between qualitative and quantitative approaches into the account, there is an anticipation to make a merge between these two. There is an enormous prospect to turn up scientists’ philanthropic innovations if they could use fuzzy logic in social science researches! However, by using fuzzy logic in sociological research there is a great deal of opportunity to study the social facts related to poverty, consumption, employment, intersubjectivity, social capital, environment, gender etc. How can we use Yager’s theory of Fuzzy Logic to analyze the relationship between social capital and labor market partcicpation? From the experiential connection in Bangladesh society, I try to seek this answer using a hypothetical quantification of attributes.

David Collier

Analysts who developed the set-theoretic comparative method (STCM) have formulated admirable goals for researchers who work in the qualitative and multi-method tradition. STCM includes above all Charles Ragin’s innovative approach of Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA). However, the analytic tools employed by STCM have in many ways become an obstacle to achieving these goals. For example, the system of fuzzy-set scoring appears to be problematic, poorly matched to a standard under-standing of conceptual structure, and perhaps unnecessary in its present form. Computer simulations suggest that findings suffer from serious problems of stability and validity; and while the choice of simulations that appropriately evaluate the method is a matter of some controversy, the cumulative weight of simulation results raises major concerns about STCM’s algorithms—i.e., its basic, formalized analytic procedures. Questions also arise about the cumbersome formulation of findings in what is often a remarkably large number of causal paths. Relatedly, some scholars question the STCM’s rejection of the parsimonious findings, in the form of “net effects,” routinely reported in other methodological traditions. Regarding applications, readily available software has encouraged publication of dozens of articles that appear to abandon key foundations of the method and rely far too heavily on these algorithms. Finally, STCM appears inattentive to the major, recent rethinking of standards and procedures for causal inference from observational data. These problems raise the concern that the set-theoretic comparative method, as applied and practiced, has become disconnected from the underlying analytic goals that motivated Charles Ragin to create it.

American Journal of Sociology

Edgar Ojeda

Sujay Rao Mandavilli

The Sociological Ninety ten rules that are proposed in this paper, are based on the fundamental premise that various branches of social sciences like sociology, anthropology and economics are human-centric and are therefore inexact, and vary fundamentally from the more precise and exact sciences like physics, chemistry and mathematics which are characterized by precision and exactitude. A high degree of precision and certainty may not therefore manifest themselves in various branches of the social sciences, even if they at times make use of mathematical models or statistical techniques. Therefore, for every postulated rule in most fields in the social sciences, there are likely to be many different exceptions. These may be described as cultural variations and cultural exceptions, and exceptions over time or space. The name 'Ninety ten' is only an easy-to-understand and easy-to-use nomenclature. Real world exceptions to any given observation could be twenty per cent, five per cent, or take on any other value, but the above nomenclature is chosen for convenience. Variations across or within cultures or within or in between socio-cultural groups, socioeconomic groups, occupational groups or any other parameter must be assessed based on the principles that we propose. This may be a basis for splitting up such groups if necessary for further study and evaluation, and the prerogative for this lies with the researcher. Thus, not only rule-based reasoning but also case-based reasoning must be used for various fields in the social sciences. Therefore, a fundamental premise of this paper is that exceptions must be sought actively, as these will lead to better research and hypothesis formulation. Thus, every researcher must think of rules and exceptions to those rules, and this must become a mindset. If exceptions are significant, they may warrant an altogether different line of research. This process will also greatly aid in inductive analysis, nomothetic rule-building and theorization, and play a major role in the 'Globalization of science', particularly social sciences.

Sociological Methods & Research

Charles Ragin , Paul Pennings

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Gary Thomas

ignoramus financio

angelo fusari

revistapuce

Pablo Garces Velastegui

elizabeth kier

Farshid Shams

PHILIPPE SCHMITTER

Yolanda Suarez-balcazar

Crossing Dialogues

Robert Henman

Abdulla Alghalai

David Arellano-Gault

In Bent Flyvbjerg, Todd Landman, and Sanford Schram, eds., Real Social Science: Applied Phronesis, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1-14

Bent Flyvbjerg

Super Kmaleon

Qualitative Inquiry, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 219-245

rocky victoria

Jorge J E Gracia

International Studies …

Valerie Hudson

Nagalaxmi Lucky

Working Papers

Ipsita Sapra

The SAGE Handbook of Case-Based Methods

Lars Mjøset

Dr. Awais H. Gillani

Marriage & Family Review

Walter Schumm

Qualitative Inquiry

christian lund

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

land-logo

Article Menu

one shot case study design research

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Author Biographies
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

A reflection on the implementation of a waterfront greenway from a social–ecological perspective: a case study of huangyan-taizhou in china.

one shot case study design research

1. Introduction

  • How are forms of land use and ecosystem services—in particular, cultural services (e.g., recreation)—impacted by the process of urbanisation?
  • What forms of everyday activities are performed in different parts of the greenway, specifically in urban and rural areas as well as the urban–rural interface?
  • For more sustainable planning and improved decision-making, how can the social–ecological perspective support a better understanding of human–nature relationships?

2. Case Selection and Methodology

2.1. case selection, 2.2. methodical frame-scheme for trans-scaled research, 2.3. methods and data collection, 3. an understanding of greenways from social and ecological interactions, 3.1. the social–ecological system, 3.2. the perspective of ecosystem services, 3.3. summary, 4. analysis of ecosystem services and yrg in huangyan-taizhou, 4.1. dynamics of land use and ecosystem services in huangyan, 4.2. yrg in the local plans for green space, 4.3. three sections of the yrg in huangyan-taizhou, 6. findings and discussion.

  • While the macro analysis of the land cover showed decreasing extents of water, forest, and grassland, the regional greenways by the river improved the living environment and enriched everyday leisure. In the field investigation, we obtained positive statements from local people regarding the ecosystem restructuring: “I don’t know if the ecosystem has improved, but I feel the green space has increased. I often take my granddaughter to play at the riverbank. A place for playing is much closer than before” (in-depth interview, October 2019).
  • At the macro scale, the greenway efficiently connects local ecological resources and provides forms of recreation for residents. At the micro scale, these recreations were not always ‘designed’, but rather the spaces were reshaped by everyday activities. For example, farmed areas in the urban–rural interface and rural sections of the greenway were initiated by local residents. According to the interviews conducted in 2023 and 2024, people stated that retaining these areas as grassland would be a waste of the riverbank.
  • Different patterns of activities could be identified. The spatial distribution of activities can be divided into three patterns: strip distribution, concentrated distribution, and scattered distribution. For instance, walking, fishing, and sightseeing are mainly distributed in strips along the river, while social activities such as dancing are concentrated in the main square. However, the distribution of parent–child interactions, picking herbs, etc. is scattered.
  • The distance between activities and water bodies also showed certain patterns. Activities closer to the water body showing a strip distribution can reflect the supply functions of the ecosystem such as fishing and washing, which are generally found along the river activity zone in a strip up to 5 m from the water body. At the same time, larger crowds are normally found within 150 m of the greenway, combined with other functions such as commercial usage.
  • People living in urban areas use the greenway for recreation much more frequently than those living in the urban–rural interface and rural areas. Similarly, people in urban areas prefer cycling in the natural environment of the greenways whilst rural residents prefer to ride electronic scooters by the river, an activity that has become restricted in the greenway.

7. Conclusions

Author contributions, data availability statement, acknowledgments, conflicts of interest.

  • Berkes, F.; Folke, C.; Colding, J. (Eds.) Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience ; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McIntosh, R.J. Social memory in Mande. In The Way the Wind Blows: Climate, History, and Human Action ; McIntosh, R.J., Tainter, J.A., McIntosh, S.K., Eds.; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 141–180. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brenner, N. The Urban Question as a Scale Question: Reflections on Henri Lefebvre, Urban Theory and the Politics of Scale. Int. J. Urban Reg. Res. 2000 , 24 , 361–378. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lefebvre, H. The Urban Revolution ; U of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brenner, N.; Katsikis, N. Operational Landscapes: Hinterlands of the Capitalocene. Archit. Des. 2020 , 90 , 22–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Jax, K.; Barton, D.N.; Chan, K.M.; De Groot, R.; Doyle, U.; Eser, U.; Görg, C.; Gómez-Baggethun, E.; Griewald, Y.; Haber, W.; et al. Ecosystem services and ethics. Ecol. Econ. 2013 , 93 , 260–268. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Soman, S.; Beyeler, S.; Kraft, S.E.; Thomas, D.; Winstanley, D. Ecosystem Services from Riparian Areas: A Brief Summary of the Literature; Prepared for the Scientific Advisory Committee of the Illinois River Coordinator Council. 2007. Available online: http://www.verdevalley.org/RiparianAreas.pdf (accessed on 1 July 2024).
  • Alberti, M.; Marzluff, J.M. Ecological resilience in urban ecosystems: Linking urban patterns to human and ecological functions. Urban Ecosyst. 2004 , 7 , 241–265. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • de La Hera-Portillo, Á.; López-Gutiérrez, J.; Zorrilla-Miras, P.; Mayor, B.; López-Gunn, E. The ecosystem resilience concept applied to hydrogeological systems: A general approach. Water 2020 , 12 , 1824. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Capon, S.J.; Chambers, L.E.; Mac Nally, R.; Naiman, R.J.; Davies, P.; Marshall, N.; Pittock, J.; Reid, M.; Capon, T.; Douglas, M.; et al. Riparian ecosystems in the 21st century: Hotspots for climate change adaptation? Ecosystems 2013 , 16 , 359–381. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Muketha, S. Riparian Zones and Their Role in Enhancing Resilience to Flooding in Urban Areas. Afr. Habitat Rev. 2019 , 13 , 1615–1625. [ Google Scholar ]
  • White, J.M.; Stromberg, J.C. Resilience, restoration, and riparian ecosystems: Case study of a dryland, urban river. Restor. Ecol. 2011 , 19 , 101–111. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Samant, S.; Brears, R. Urban waterfront revivals of the future. In Greening Cities ; Advances in 21st Century Human Settlements; Tan, P., Jim, C., Eds.; Springer: Singapore, 2017; pp. 15–33. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Anderson, A.B.; Jenkins, C.N. Applying Nature’s Design: Corridors as a Strategy for Biodiversity Conservation ; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhiqiang, W.; Liu, X.C.; Zhao, G. Replacing expansion-oriented with efficiency-led spatial goals: Key evaluation indicators for urban governance. In Urban Planning Forum ; Tongji University Press: Shanghai, China, 2021; Volume 5, pp. 15–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tang, Y.; Li, X.; Hu, X.; Liu, X. Evolution of the scale and influencing factors of small towns in traditional agricultural areas under rapid urbanization. Econ. Geogr. 2023 , 43 , 148–159. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ahern, J. Greenways as Strategic Landscape Planning: Theory and Application ; Wageningen University and Research: Wageningen, The Netherlands, 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jongman, R.H.; Pungetti, G. (Eds.) Ecological Networks and Greenways: Concept, Design, Implementation ; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shi, W.; Woolley, H. Managing for multifunctionality in urban open spaces: Approaches for sustainable development. J. Urban Manag. 2014 , 3 , 3–21. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tedesco, A.M.; López-Cubillos, S.; Chazdon, R.; Rhodes, J.R.; Archibald, C.L.; Pérez-Hämmerle, K.V.; Dean, A.J. Beyond ecology: Ecosystem restoration as a process for social-ecological transformation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2023 , 38 , 643–653. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Walker, B.; Holling, C.S.; Carpenter, S.R.; Kinzig, A. Resilience, adaptability and transformability in social-ecological systems. Ecol. Soc. 2004 , 9 , 5. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Reyers, B.; Folke, C.; Moore, M.L.; Biggs, R.; Galaz, V. Social-ecological systems insights for navigating the dynamics of the Anthropocene. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2018 , 43 , 267–289. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Berkes, F.; Kislalioglu, M.; Folke, C.; Gadgil, M. Minireviews: Exploring the basic ecological unit: Ecosystem-like concepts in traditional societies. Ecosystems 1998 , 1 , 409–415. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ostrom, E. A general framework for analyzing sustainability of social-ecological systems. Science 2009 , 325 , 419–422. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McGinnis, M.D.; Ostrom, E. Social-ecological system framework: Initial changes and continuing challenges. Ecol. Soc. 2014 , 19 , 30. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Peng, Y.; Zhang, Q.; Chen, Y.C.; Ning, X.U.; Qiao, Y.; Tian, C.; Li, F. Resilience, adaptability, and regime shifts thinking: A perspective of dryland socio-ecology system. J. Resour. Ecol. 2021 , 12 , 376–383. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Partelow, S. A review of the social-ecological systems framework. Ecol. Soc. 2018 , 23 , 36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Aminpour, P.; Gray, S.A.; Jetter, A.J.; Introne, J.E.; Singer, A.; Arlinghaus, R. Wisdom of stakeholder crowds in complex social–ecological systems. Nat. Sustain. 2020 , 3 , 191–199. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhu, W.; Zhang, Z.; Zhang, H.; Tovuudorj, R. Integrating spatial patterns and driving factors of cultural ecosystem services into territorial spatial governance: A case study of the Horqin Sandy Land with multi-ethnic settlements. Habitat Int. 2024 , 148 , 103093. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Xia, H.; Yuan, S.; Prishchepov, A.V. Spatial-temporal heterogeneity of ecosystem service interactions and their social-ecological drivers: Implications for spatial planning and management. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2023 , 189 , 106767. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Grunewald, K.; Bastian, O.; Louda, J.; Arcidiacono, A.; Brzoska, P.; Bue, M.; Cetin, N.I.; Dworczyk, C.; Dubova, L.; Fitch, A.; et al. Lessons learned from implementing the ecosystem services concept in urban planning. Ecosyst. Serv. 2021 , 49 , 101273. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shen, J.; Li, S.; Wang, H.; Wu, S.; Liang, Z.; Zhang, Y.; Wei, F.; Li, S.; Ma, L.; Wang, Y.; et al. Understanding the spatial relationships and drivers of ecosystem service supply-demand mismatches towards spatially-targeted management of social-ecological. system. J. Clean. Prod. 2023 , 406 , 136882. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, R.; Browning, M.H.; Kee, F.; Hunter, R.F. Exploring mechanistic pathways linking urban green and blue space to mental wellbeing before and after urban regeneration of a greenway: Evidence from the Connswater Community Greenway, Belfast, UK. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2023 , 235 , 104739. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Millennium Ecosystem Assessment. Ecosystems and Human Well-Being: Wetlands and Water Synthesis ; World Resources Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 2005; ISBN 1-56973-597-2. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quintas-Soriano, C.; Brandt, J.S.; Running, K.; Baxter, C.V.; Gibson, D.M.; Narducci, J.; Castro, A.J. Social-ecological systems influence ecosystem service perception. Ecol. Soc. 2018 , 23 , 3. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chen, C.; Deng, W.; Ignatieva, M.; Bi, L.; Du, A.; Yang, L. Synergy of urban green space planning and ecosystem services provision: A longitudinal exploration of China’s development. Urban For. Urban Green. 2023 , 86 , 127997. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Plieninger, T.; Dijks, S.; Oteros-Rozas, E.; Bieling, C. Assessing, mapping, and quantifying cultural ecosystem services at community level. Land Use Policy 2013 , 33 , 118–129. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhao, Y.; You, W.; Lin, X.; He, D. Assessing the supply and demand linkage of cultural ecosystem services in a typical county-level city with protected areas in China. Ecol. Indic. 2023 , 147 , 109992. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Andersson, E.; Nykvist, B.; Malinga, R.; Jaramillo, F.; Lindborg, R. A social–ecological analysis of ecosystem services in two different farming systems. Ambio 2015 , 44 , 102–112. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ghasemi, M.; Charrahy, Z.; González-García, A. Mapping cultural ecosystem services provision: An integrated model of recreation and ecotourism opportunities. Land Use Policy 2023 , 131 , 106732. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Brown, G.; Pullar, D.; Hausner, V.H. An empirical evaluation of spatial value transfer methods for identifying cultural ecosystem services. Ecol. Indic. 2016 , 69 , 1–11. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Müller, S.M.; Peisker, J.; Bieling, C.; Linnemann, K.; Reidl, K.; Schmieder, K. The importance of cultural ecosystem services and biodiversity for landscape visitors in the biosphere reserve Swabian Alb (Germany). Sustainability 2019 , 11 , 2650. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Weber, S.; Boley, B.B.; Palardy, N.; Gaither, C.J. The impact of urban greenways on residential concerns: Findings from the Atlanta BeltLine Trail. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017 , 167 , 147–156. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yu, K.; Li, D.; Li, N. The evolution of greenways in China. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2006 , 76 , 223–239. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Flink, C.A.; Searns, R.M. Greenways: A Guide to Planning, Design, and Development ; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ahern, J. Greenways as a planning strategy. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1995 , 33 , 131–155. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hellmund, P.C.; Smith, D. Designing Greenways: Sustainable Landscapes for Nature and People ; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walmsley, A. Greenways and the making of urban form. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1995 , 33 , 81–127. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • President’s Commission on Americans Outdoors (US). Americans Outdoors: The Legacy, the Challenge, with Case Studies: The Report of the President’s Commission ; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Larson, K.L.; Nelson, K.C.; Samples, S.R.; Hall, S.J.; Bettez, N.; Cavender-Bares, J.; Groffman, P.M.; Grove, M.; Heffernan, J.B.; Hobbie, S.E.; et al. Ecosystem services in managing residential landscapes: Priorities, value dimensions, and cross-regional patterns. Urban Ecosyst. 2016 , 19 , 95–113. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Fabos, J.G. Greenway planning in the United States: Its origins and recent case studies. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2004 , 68 , 321–342. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bryant, M.M. Urban landscape conservation and the role of ecological greenways at local and metropolitan scales. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2006 , 76 , 23–44. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • White, J.T. Pursuing design excellence: Urban design governance on Toronto’s waterfront. Prog. Plan. 2016 , 110 , 1–41. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Xiu, N.; Ignatieva, M.; van den Bosch, C.K.; Chai, Y.; Wang, F.; Cui, T.; Yang, F. A socio-ecological perspective of urban green networks: The Stockholm case. Urban Ecosyst. 2017 , 20 , 729–742. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Huang, H.; Xiao, S.; Fügener, T.; Wende, W.; Haase, A.M. Land transition and socio-ecological restructuring at the urban-rural interface: The case of Huangyan-Taizhou. Dialogues 2023 , 2023 , 68–71. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lambin, E.F.; Geist, H.J.; Lepers, E. Dynamics of land-use and land-cover change in tropical regions. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2003 , 28 , 205–241. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lambin, E.F.; Meyfroidt, P. Land use transitions: Socio-ecological feedback versus socio-economic change. Land Use Policy 2010 , 27 , 108–118. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]

Click here to enlarge figure

The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Huang, H.; Qi, J.; Xiao, S.; Wende, W.; Xin, Y. A Reflection on the Implementation of a Waterfront Greenway from a Social–Ecological Perspective: A Case Study of Huangyan-Taizhou in China. Land 2024 , 13 , 989. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13070989

Huang H, Qi J, Xiao S, Wende W, Xin Y. A Reflection on the Implementation of a Waterfront Greenway from a Social–Ecological Perspective: A Case Study of Huangyan-Taizhou in China. Land . 2024; 13(7):989. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13070989

Huang, Huang, Jiaxin Qi, Suili Xiao, Wolfgang Wende, and Yingdu Xin. 2024. "A Reflection on the Implementation of a Waterfront Greenway from a Social–Ecological Perspective: A Case Study of Huangyan-Taizhou in China" Land 13, no. 7: 989. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13070989

Article Metrics

Further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

IMAGES

  1. The One-Shot Case Study

    one shot case study design research

  2. Experimental Research

    one shot case study design research

  3. PPT

    one shot case study design research

  4. One Shot Case Study Design research design

    one shot case study design research

  5. PPT

    one shot case study design research

  6. What is a one-shot experimental case study (pre-experimental research design)??

    one shot case study design research

VIDEO

  1. One shot case complete 😎😎...#trendingreels #kannadaquotes #nammabelagavi..#lovedancer

  2. Case Study Design Group 2

  3. BCBA Task List 5: D 4

  4. Implementation Science Education Series Seminar 5: Outcomes and Measures

  5. Clinical Study Design Part 1

  6. Yin (2003) Case Study Research pt 2 Research strategies compared

COMMENTS

  1. Pre-Experimental Designs

    One-group pretest-posttest design; Static-group comparison; One-shot case study design. A single group is studied at a single point in time after some treatment that is presumed to have caused change. The carefully studied single instance is compared to general expectations of what the case would have looked like had the treatment not occurred ...

  2. One-Group Posttest Only Design: An Introduction

    The one-group posttest-only design (a.k.a. one-shot case study) is a type of quasi-experiment in which the outcome of interest is measured only once after exposing a non-random group of participants to a certain intervention.. The objective is to evaluate the effect of that intervention which can be: A training program; A policy change; A medical treatment, etc.

  3. The One-Shot Case Study

    The one-shot case study design is shown in figure 4.1e.It is also called the ex post facto design because a single group of people is measured on some dependent variable after an intervention has taken place.. This is the most common research design in culture change studies, where it is obviously impossible to manipulate the dependent variable.

  4. PDF Chapter 9: Experimental Research

    1. One-Shot Case Study Design a. Also called the one group posttest-only design, the one-shot case study design has only one group, a treatment, and a posttest. Because there is only one group, there is no random assignment. A weakness of this design is that it is difficult to say for sure that the treatment caused the dependent variable. If ...

  5. Single-Case Design, Analysis, and Quality Assessment for Intervention

    Keywords: n-of-one studies, single-subject research, research design, quality assessment. Introduction. The purpose of this article is to present current tools and techniques relevant for single-case rehabilitation research. Single-case (SC) studies have been identified by a variety of names, including "n of 1 studies" and "single-subject ...

  6. Pre-Experimental Designs

    One-group pretest-posttest design; Static-group comparison; One-shot case study design. A single group is studied at a single point in time after some treatment that is presumed to have caused change. The carefully studied single instance is compared to general expectations of what the case would have looked like had the treatment not occurred ...

  7. Role of Single Group Studies in Agency for Healthcare Research and

    A classification scheme developed by Campbell and Stanley describes two single group studies consistent with our definition: the "one-shot case study" and the "one-group pretest-post-test design." 2 In the one-shot case study, a single group is studied only once after a treatment is applied. In the one-group pretest-post-test design ...

  8. Design Case Study

    The one-shot case study design, often referred to as the posttest-only design, examines a single group on a single occasion. ... Yet, such a design adds compelling knowledge to guide further research and practice. One-shot case study and static group comparisons are most useful in answering descriptive questions such as "What happened after a ...

  9. PDF Chapter 8. Experiments Topics Appropriate for Experimental Research

    What is "One-shot case study?" Characteristics of one-shot case study No control group, only experimental group No pre-test Compare the result with some intuitive standard An example: One wants to determine whether reading to children an extra ½ ho ur a day would increase their reading skill. A group of children are chosen. The teacher ...

  10. What is Pre-Experimental Design?

    In a one-shot case study, a single group or case is studied at a single point in time after some intervention or treatment that is presumed to cause change. One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design. In the one-group pretest-posttest design, a single group is observed at two time points, one before the treatment and one after the treatment. Static ...

  11. PDF Single Case Design

    3 7 One Group Posttest Only Design X (IV) O (DV) Also called a "one shot case study" Example: This class, and this class only, attends a 2 hour lecture on Validity and then immediately takes a test to gauge Validity knowledge What is wrong with this design? - No comparison group

  12. Experimental Research Designs

    Experimental Research Designs. R=Random Assignment X= Treatment O=Observation (Assessment) X O One Shot Case Study Design O X O One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design X O Static-Group Comparis ...

  13. Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

    The pre-experimental research design is further divided into three types. One-shot Case Study Research Design. In this type of experimental study, only one dependent group or variable is considered. The study is carried out after some treatment which was presumed to cause change, making it a posttest study.

  14. 12.2: Pre-experimental and quasi-experimental design

    One-shot case study- a pre-experimental design that applies an intervention to only one group without a pretest. Pre-experimental designs- a variation of experimental design that lacks the rigor of experiments and is often used before a true experiment is conducted. Quasi-experimental design- designs lack random assignment to experimental and ...

  15. Pre-experimental design: Definition, types & examples

    The research design can still be useful for exploratory research to test the feasibility for further study. ... A one-shot case study design only analyses post-test results. The one-shot case study compares the post-test results to the expected results. It makes clear what the result is and how the case would have looked if the treatment wasn ...

  16. (PDF) Research Designs

    A research design is defined as the series of those steps . ... to the One-Shot Case Study as this design enables the . comparison of the dependent variable(s) before and . after treatment.

  17. Experimental and Quasi-experimental Designs for Research

    the three preexperimental designs discussed are: (1) the one-shot case study; (2) the one-group pretest-posttest design; and (3) the static-group comparison. ... correlational and expost facto designs are discussed as research design with a control factor not characteristic of the other quasi-experimental designs, but insufficient to qualify as ...

  18. PDF Experimental Designs

    One shot case study X O 2. One group pretest-posttest design O X O 7 . III. Experimental Designs (Cont.) ... Sampling and survey research 13 . NEXT WEEK: Single-case design 14 . Author: Chicocat Created Date: 4/2/2012 10:07:01 AM ...

  19. Good example one shot case study research?

    Mary C R Wilson. Hello, This article available on ResearchGate uses one shot case study as its research design: Mongkuo, M. Y., & Quantrell, S. (2015) The Influence of Excessive Alcohol ...

  20. APA Dictionary of Psychology

    one-shot case study. a research design in which a single group is observed on a single occasion after experiencing some event, treatment, or intervention. Because there is no control group against which to make comparisons, it is a weak design; any changes noted are merely presumed to have been caused by the event.

  21. Pre-experimental one-shot case study design.

    This study employed a cross-sectional pre-experimental one- shot case study design [49]. A schematic representation of the design is displayed in Figure 1. where X is a PBC student's types and ...

  22. 2. The "One Shot" Case Study Revisited

    The "One Shot" Case Study Revisited. 2. The "One Shot" Case Study Revisited. Gary King. Method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.———.(2000). Fuzzy Set Social Science. ... by using fuzzy logic in sociological research there is a great deal of opportunity to study the social facts related to poverty, consumption, employment ...

  23. The impact of evidence-based nursing leadership in healthcare settings

    Design. In this review, we used a mixed methods approach [].A mixed methods systematic review was selected as this approach has the potential to produce direct relevance to policy makers and practitioners [].Johnson and Onwuegbuzie [] have defined mixed methods research as "the class of research in which the researcher mixes or combines quantitative and qualitative research techniques ...

  24. Services

    Case studies ; Services . What we do ... Deloitte's ongoing focus on research and development (R&D) is what has inspired us to carry out this survey - our first research project of this kind since the outbreak of COVID-19 in 2020. ... Deloitte refers to one or more of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited, a UK private company limited by ...

  25. The one-shot case study design

    This study used experimental methods with one-shot case study design. The population of this study was class X SMK Pelayaran Wira Samudera Semarang. The sample was 30 students selected...

  26. Land

    In recent years, awareness has grown of the vital importance of ecological systems, provoking increased research into how to improve their resilience. Here, one popular new technical/management solution is the creation of greenways along riversides. In practice, however, the practical outcomes of such greenways have sometimes been disappointing due to an excessive focus on technical solutions ...